Oxford University Press - Online Resource Centres

Mackintosh: IQ and Human Intelligence 2e

Hyperlinked bibliography

A - B - C - D - E - F - G - H - I - J - K - L - M - N - O - P - Q - R - S - T - U - V - W - X - Y - Z

Please note that some of the sites you will be directed to from this page require a username and password to access the article.

Ackerman, P. L. (1988). Determinants of individual differences during skill acquisition: Cognitive abilities and information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 299–318.

Ackerman, P. L. and Lohman, D. F. (2003). Education and g. In The scientific study of general intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen (ed. H. Nyborg). Oxford: Pergamon.

Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M. E., and Boyle, M. O. (2005). Working memory and intelligence: The same or different constructs? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 30–60 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.30] [PubMed: 15631550].

Ackroyd, P. (1990). Dickens. London: Sinclair-Stevenson.

Adler, J. E. and Rips, L. J. (2008). Reasoning: Studies of human inference and its foundations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Aiello, L. C. and Dean, M. C. (1990). An introduction to human evolutionary anatomy. London: Academic Press.

Alcock, K. J. and Bundy, D. A. P. (2001). The impact of infectious disease on cognitive development. In Environmental effects on cognitive abilities (eds R. J. Sternberg and E. L. Grigorenko). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Aleman, A., Bronk, E., Kessels, R. P. C., Koppeschaar, H. P. P., and van Honk, J. (2004). A single administration of testosterone improves visuospatial ability in young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 612–17 [DOI: 10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00089-1] [PubMed: 15041084].

Alexander, J. R. M. and Smales, S. (1997). Intelligence, learning and long term memory. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 815–25 [DOI: 10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00054-8].

Anastasi, A. and Urbina, S. (2001). Psychological testing (7th edn). London: Prentice-Hall International.

Anderson, B. (2001). g as a consequence of shared genes. Intelligence, 29, 367–71 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00069-1].

Anderson, J. W., Johnstone, B. M., and Remley, D. T. (1999). Breast-feeding and cognitive development: A meta-analysis. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 70, 525–35 [PubMed: 10500022].

Anderson, M. (1992). Intelligence and development: A cognitive theory. Oxford: Blackwell.

Anderson, M. (2005). Marrying intelligence and cognition: A developmental view. In Cognition and intelligence: Identifying the mechanisms of the mind (eds R.  J. Sternberg and J. E. Pretz). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Arden, R. and Plomin, R. (2006). Sex differences in variance of intelligence across childhood. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 39–48 [DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.027].

Ardila, A., Galeano, L. M., and Rosselli, M. (1998). Toward a model of neuropsychological activity. Neuropsychology Review, 8, 171–90 [DOI: 10.1023/A:1021618218943] [PubMed: 9951709].

Ardila, A., Pineda, D., and Rosselli, M. (2000). Correlation between intelligence test scores and executive function measures. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 31–6 [DOI: 10.1016/S0887-6177(98)00159-0] [PubMed: 14590565].

Atkinson, R. (2007). College admissions and the SAT: A personal perspective. In R. C. Atkinson, The pursuit of knowledge: Speeches and papers of Richard C. Atkinson. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.

Attridge, L. C. (2006). The role of emotional intelligence in academic achievement and its relation to other measures of individual differences. Unpublished M.Phil.Thesis, University of Cambridge.

Back to top

Baddeley, A. D. (1997) Human memory: Theory and practice. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

Baddeley, A. D. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Baddeley, A. D. and Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In Recent advances in learning and motivation (vol. 8.), (ed. G. A. Bower). New York: Academic Press.

Baddeley, A. D. and Wilson, B. (1988). Frontal amnesia and the dysexecutive syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 7, 212–30 [DOI: 10.1016/0278-2626(88)90031-0] [PubMed: 3377900].

Baenninger, M. and Newcombe, N. S. (1989). The role of experience in spatial test-performance: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 20, 327–44 [DOI: 10.1007/BF00287729].

Baldo, J. V. and Shimamura, A. P. (1998). Letter and category fluency in patients with frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychology, 12, 259–67 [DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.12.2.259] [PubMed: 9556772].

Baldo, J. V. and Shimamura, A. P. (2000). Spatial and color working memory in patients with lateral prefrontal cortex lesions. Psychobiology, 28, 156–67.

Baltes, P. B. and Lindenberger, U. (1997). Emergence of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: A new window to the study of cognitive aging? Psychology and Aging, 12, 12–21 [DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.12.1.12] [PubMed: 9100264].

Banks, G. C., Batchelor, J. H., and McDaniel, M. A. (2010). Smarter people are (a bit) more symmetrical: A meta-analysis of the relationship between intelligence and fluctuating asymmetry. Intelligence, 38, 393–401 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2010.04.003].

Barnett, W. S. (1995). Long-term effects of early childhood programs on cognitive and school outcomes. The Future of Children, 5 (3), 25–50 [DOI: 10.2307/1602366].

Barnett, W. S. and Masse, L. N. (2007). Early childhood program design and economic returns: Comparative benefit-cost analysis of the Abecedarian program and policy implications. Economics of Education Review, 26, 113–25 [DOI: 10.1016/j.econedurev.2005.10.007].

Bar-On, R. (1997). The Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-i): A test of emotional intelligence. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.

Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mind blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Baron-Cohen, S. (2003). The essential difference: Men, women and the extreme male brain. London: Penguin.

Baron-Cohen, S., Lutchmaya, S., and Knickmeyer, R. (2004). Prenatal testosterone in mind: Amniotic fluid studies. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Barrett, P. T. and Eysenck, H. J. (1992). Brain-evoked potentials and intelligence: the Hendrickson paradigm. Intelligence, 16, 361–81 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(92)90015-J].

Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., and Judge, T. A. (2001). Personality and performance at the beginning of the new millennium: What do we know and where do we go next? International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 9, 9–30 [DOI: 10.1111/1468-2389.00160].

Bartholomew, D. J. (2004). Measuring intelligence: Facts and fallacies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Bartholomew, D. J., Deary, I. J., and Lawn, M. (2009). A new lease of life for Thomson’s bonds model of intelligence. Psychological Review, 116, 567–79 [DOI: 10.1037/a0016262] [PubMed: 19618987].

Bartley, A. J., Jones, D. W., and Weinberger, D. R. (1997). Genetic variability of human brain size and cortical gyral patterns. Brain, 120, 257–69 [DOI: 10.1093/brain/120.2.257] [PubMed: 9117373].

Bastian, V. A., Burns, N. R., and Nettelbeck, T. (2005). Emotional intelligence predicts life skills, but not as well as personality and cognitive abilities. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 1135–45 [DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.04.006].

Bates, T. C. and Eysenck, H. J. (1993a). String length, attention and intelligence: focused attention reverses the string-length-IQ relationship. Personality and Individual Differences, 15, 363–371 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(93)90063-9].

Bates, T. C. and Eysenck, H. J. (1993b). Intelligence, inspection time and decision time. Intelligence, 17, 523–32 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(93)90016-X].

Bates, T. C., Stough, C., Mangan, G., and Pellett, O. (1995). Intelligence and complexity of the averaged evoked potential: An attentional theory. Intelligence, 20, 27–39 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(95)90004-7].

Baumeister, A. A. and Bacharach, V. R. (1996). A critical analysis of the infant health and development programme. Intelligence, 23, 79–104 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(96)90007-0].

Bayley, N. (1969). The Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New York: Psychological Corporation.

Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Development (2nd edn). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Beller, E. and Hout, M. (2006). Intergenerational social mobility: The United States in comparative perspective. The Future of Children, 16 (2), 19–36 [DOI: 10.1353/foc.2006.0012] [PubMed: 17036544].

Bellinger, D. C. and Adams, H. F. (2001). Environmental pollutant exposures and children’s cognitive abilities. In Environmental effects on cognitive abilities (eds R. J. Sternberg and E. L. Grigorenko). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Bellis, M. A., Hughes, K., Hughes, S., and Ashton, J. R. (2005). Measuring paternal discrepancy and its public health consequences. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 59, 749–54 [DOI: 10.1136/jech.2005.036517] [PubMed: 16100312].

Belmont, L. and Marolla, F. A. (1973). Birth order, family size, and intelligence. Science, 182, 1096–101 [DOI: 10.1126/science.182.4117.1096] [PubMed: 4750607].

Benbow, C. P. (1988). Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability in intellectually talented preadolescents: Their nature, effects, and possible causes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 169–232 [DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X00049244].

Benbow, C. P. and Stanley, J. C. (1983). Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability: More facts. Science, 222, 1029–31 [DOI: 10.1126/science.6648516] [PubMed: 6648516].

Bench, C. J., Frith, C. D., Grasby, P. M., and Friston, K. J. (1993). Investigations of the functional anatomy of attention using the Stroop test. Neuropsychologia, 31, 907–22 [DOI: 10.1016/0028-3932(93)90147-R] [PubMed: 8232848].

Bennett, H. L. (1983). Remembering drink orders: the memory skills of cocktail waitresses. Human Learning: Journal of Practical Research and Applications, 2, 157–70.

Bennetto, L., Taylor, A. K., Pennington, B. F., Porter, D., and Hagerman, R. J. (2001). Profile of cognitive functioning in women with the fragile X mutation. Neuropsychology, 15, 290–9 [DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.15.2.290] [PubMed: 11324870].

Benson, N., Hulac, D. M., and Kranzler, J. H. (2010). Independent examination of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV): What does the WAIS-IV measure? Psychological Assessment, 22, 121–30 [DOI: 10.1037/a0017767] [PubMed: 20230158].

Benton, D. and Buts, J. (1990). Vitamin-mineral supplementation and intelligence. Lancet, 335, 1158–60 [DOI: 10.1016/0140-6736(90)91161-3] [PubMed: 1971890].

Benton, D. and Roberts, G. (1988). Effect of vitamin and mineral supplementation on intelligence of a sample of schoolchildren. Lancet, 331, 140–44 [DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(88)92720-1].

Berry, D. C. and Broadbent, D. E. (1984). On the relationship between task performance and associated verbalisable knowledge. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36A, 209–31.

Bilalić, M., McLeod, P., and Gobet, F. (2007). Does chess need intelligence? A study with young chess players. Intelligence, 35, 457–70 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.09.005].

Binet, A. (1900). Recherches sur la technique de la mensuration de la tête vivante, plus 4 autres mémoires sur la céphalométrie. L’Année Psychologique, 7, 314–429 [DOI: 10.3406/psy.1900.3221].

Binet, A. (1911). Nouvelles recherches sur la mésure du niveau intellectual chez les enfants d’école. L’AnnéeAnnee Psychologique, 17, 145–201.

Binet, A., and Simon, T. (1905). Méthodes nouvelles pour le diagnostic du niveau intellectuel des anormaux. L’Année Psychologique, 11, 191–244.

Binet, A., and Simon, T. (1911). A method of measuring the development of the intelligence of young children. Lincoln, IL: Courier Company.

Birch, H. G., Pineiro, C., Alcalde, E., Toca, T., and Cravioto, J. (1971). Kwashiorkor in early childhood and intelligence at school age. Pediatric Research, 5, 579–84.

Birren, J. E. (1968). Increments and decrements in the intellectual status of the aged. Psychiatric Research Report, 23, 207–14.

Bishop, S. J., Fossella, J., Croucher, C. J., and Duncan, J. (2008). COMT val158met genotype affects recruitment of neural mechanisms supporting fluid intelligence. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 2132–40 [DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhm240] [PubMed: 18252743].

Bjerkedal, T., Kristensen, P., Skjeret, G. A., and Brevik, J. I. (2007). Intelligence test scores and birth order among young Norwegian men (conscripts) analyzed within and between families. Intelligence, 35, 503–14 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2007.01.004].

Blair, C. (2006). How similar are fluid cognition and general intelligence? A developmental neuroscience perspective on fluid cognition as an aspect of human cognitive ability. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 109–60 [PubMed: 16606477].

Blair, C., Gamson, D., Thorne, S., and Baker, D. (2005). Rising mean IQ: Cognitive demand of mathematics education for young children, population exposure to formal schooling, and the neurobiology of the prefrontal cortex. Intelligence, 33, 93–106 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.008].

Blake, J. (1981). Family size and the quality of children. Demography, 18, 421–42 [DOI: 10.2307/2060941] [PubMed: 7308532].

Blinkhorn, S. F. (1995). Burt and the early history of factor analysis. In Cyril Burt: Fraud or framed? (ed. N. J. Mackintosh). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Blinkhorn, S. F. and Hendrickson, D. E. (1982). Average evoked responses and psychometric intelligence. Nature, 195, 596–97 [DOI: 10.1038/195596a0].

Block, N. J. and Dworkin, G. (1976). The IQ controversy. New York: Pantheon Books.

Bloom, B. S. (1964). Stability and change in human characteristics. New York: Wiley.

Boles, D. B. (1980). X-linkage of spatial ability: A critical review. Child Development, 51, 623–35.

Bopp, K. L. and Verhaeghen, P. (2005). Aging and verbal memory span: A meta-analysis. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 60, 223–33.

Bors, D. A. and MacLeod, C. M. (1996). Attention, information processing, and IQ. International Journal of Psychology, 31, 34–52.

Boswell, J. (1988). The kindness of strangers. New York: Pantheon Books.

Bouchard, T. J. Jr. (1997). IQ similarity in twins reared apart: findings and responses to critics. In Intelligence: Heredity and environment (eds R. J. Sternberg and E. L. Grigorenko). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Bouchard, T. J. Jr. and McGue, M. (1981). Familial studies of intelligence: A review. Science, 212, 1055–9 [DOI: 10.1126/science.7195071] [PubMed: 7195071].

Bouchard, T. J. Jr. and Segal, N. L. (1985). Environment and IQ. In Handbook of intelligence: Theories, measurements and applications (ed. B. B. Wolman). New York: Wiley.

Bouchard, T. J. Jr, Lykken, D. T., McGue, M., Segal, N. L., and Tellegen, A. (1990). Sources of human psychological differences: The Minnesota study of twins reared apart. Science, 250, 223–8 [DOI: 10.1126/science.2218526] [PubMed: 2218526].

Bowles, S. and Gintis, H. (1976). Schooling in capitalist America: Educational reform and the contradictions of economic life. New York: Harper and Row.

Bowling, A. C. and Mackenzie, B. D. (1996). The relationship between speed of information processing and cognitive ability. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 775–800 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(96)00022-0].

Brackett, M. A. and Mayer, J. D. (2003). Convergent, discriminant, and incremental validity of competing measures of emotional intelligence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1147–58 [DOI: 10.1177/0146167203254596] [PubMed: 15189610].

Brackett, M. A., Mayer, J. D., and Warner, R. M. (2004). Emotional intelligence and its relation to everyday behaviour. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1387–402 [DOI: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00236-8].

Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., Shiffman, S., Lerner, N., and Salovey, P. (2006). Relating emotional abilities to social functioning: A comparison of self-report and performance measures of emotional intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 780–95 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.780] [PubMed: 17014299].

Bradley, R. H., Caldwell, B., and Elardo, R. (1977). Home environment, social status, and mental test performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 697–701 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.69.6.697].

Brand, C. R. and Deary, I. J. (1982). Intelligence and ‘inspection time’. In A model for intelligence (ed. H. J. Eysenck). New York: Springer.

Brand, C. R., Constales, D., and Kane, H. (2003). Why ignore the g factor? Historical considerations. In The scientific study of general intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen (ed. H. Nyborg). Oxford: Pergamon.

Brandt, J., Welsh, K. A., Breitner, J. C., and Folstein, M. F. (1993). Hereditary influences on cognitive functioning in older men: A study of 4000 twin pairs. Archives of Neurology, 50, 599–603 [PubMed: 8503796].

Braver, T. S., Cohen, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Jonides, J., et  al. (1997). A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory. NeuroImage, 5, 49–62 [DOI: 10.1006/nimg.1996.0247] [PubMed: 9038284].

Brazzelli, M., Colombo, N., Della Sala, S., and Spinnler, H. (1994). Spared and impaired cognitive abilities after bilateral frontal damage. Cortex, 30, 27–51 [PubMed: 8004988].

Breslau, N., Del Detto, J. E., Brown, G. G., et  al. (1994). A gradient relationship between low birth weight and IQ at age 6 years. Archives of Adolescent Medicine, 148, 377–83.

Brewer, N. and Smith, G. A. (1984). How normal and retarded individuals monitor and regulate speed and accuracy of responding in serial choice tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 71–93 [DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.113.1.71].

Brigham, C. C. (1923). A study of American intelligence. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Brody, N. (1992). Intelligence (2nd edn). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Brody, N. (2003). Jensen’s genetic interpretation of racial differences in intelligence: Critical evaluation. In The scientific study of general intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen (ed. H. Nyborg). Oxford: Pergamon.

Broman, S. H., Nichols, P. L., and Kennedy, W. A. (1975). . In Preschool IQ: Prenatal and Early Developmental Correlates. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Brown, G. D. A. and Hulme, C. (1995). Modeling item length effects in memory span: no rehearsal needed? Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 594–621 [DOI: 10.1006/jmla.1995.1027].

Brown, R. P. and Day, E. A. (2006). The difference isn’t black and white: Stereotype threat and the race gap on Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 979–85 [DOI: 10.1037/0021-9010.91.4.979] [PubMed: 16834521].

Bryant, P. E. and Trabasso, T. (2001). Transitive inferences and memory in young children. Nature, 232, 456–8 [DOI: 10.1038/232456a0].

Buchsbaum, B. R., Greer, S., Chang, W.-L., and Berman, K. F. (2005). Meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of the Wisconsin Card-Sorting Task and component processes. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 35–45 [DOI: 10.1002/hbm.20128] [PubMed: 15846821].

Bunting, M. (2006). Proactive interference and item similarity in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 183–96 [DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.2.183].

Burkam, D. T., Ready, D. D., Lee, V. E., and LoGerfo, L. F. (2004). Social-class differences in summer learning between kindergarten and first grade: Model specification and estimation. Sociology of Education, 77, 1–31 [DOI: 10.1177/003804070407700101].

Burke, H. R. (1985). Raven’s Progressive Matrices (1938): More on norms, reliability and validity. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 72, 247–51.

Burks, B. S. (1928). The relative influence of nature and nurture upon mental development: A comparative study of foster parent-foster child resemblance and true parent-true child resemblance. Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education, 27. Bloomingdale, IL: Public School Publishing.

Burleigh, M. (1994). Death and deliverance: ‘Euthanasia’ In in Germany c.1900–1945. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Burleigh, M. (2000). The Third Reich: A new history. Macmillan: London.

Burns, N. R. and Nettelbeck, T. (2003). Inspection time in the structure of cognitive abilities: Where does IT fit? Intelligence, 31, 237–55 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00120-4].

Burns, N. R. and Nettelbeck, T. (2005). Inspection time and speed of processing: Sex differences on perceptual speed but not IT. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 439–46 [DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.01.022].

Burns, N. R., Nettelbeck, T., and Cooper, C. J. (1999). Inspection time correlates with general speed of processing but not with fluid ability. Intelligence, 27, 37–44 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(99)00013-6].

Buros, O. K. (1935). Mental Measurements Yearbook. Highland Park, NJ: Gryphon Press.

Burt, C. L. (1912). The inheritance of mental characters. Eugenics Review, 4, 168–200 [PubMed: 21259537].

Burt, C. L. (1917). The distribution and relations of educational abilities. London: London County Council.

Burt, C. L. (1921). Mental and scholastic tests. London: King and Son.

Burt, C. L. (1955). The evidence for the concept of intelligence. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 25, 158–177.

Burt, C. L. (1966). The genetic determination of differences in intelligence: a study of monozygotic twins reared together and apart. British Journal of Psychology, 57, 137–53 [PubMed: 5949392].

Burt, C. L. (1969) Intelligence and heredity: some common misconceptions. Irish Journal of Education, 3, 75–94.

Burt, C. L., and Moore, R. C. (1912). The mental differences between the sexes. Journal of Experimental Pedagogy, 1, 273–84 and 355–88.

Butcher, L. M., Davis, O. S. P., Craig, I. W., and Plomin, R. (2008). Genome-wide quantitative trait locus association scan of general cognitive ability using pooled DNA and 500K single nucleotide polymorphism microarrays. Genes, Brain, & Behavior, 7, 435–46 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00368.x].

Byrne, R. W. and Whiten, A. (1988). Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes and humans. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Back to top

Cahan, S. and Cohen, N. (1989). Age versus schooling effects on intelligence development. Child Development, 60, 1239–49 [DOI: 10.2307/1130797] [PubMed: 2805901].

Campbell, F. A., Ramey, C. T., Pungello, E., Sparling, J., and Miller-Johnson, S. (2002). Early childhood education: Young adult outcomes from the Abecedarian Project. Applied Developmental Science, 6, 42–57 [DOI: 10.1207/S1532480XADS0601_05].

Capron, C. and Duyme, M. (1989). Assessment of effects of socioeconomic status on IQ in a full cross-fostering design. Nature, 340, 552–3 [DOI: 10.1038/340552a0].

Carlson, J. S., Jensen, C. M., and Widaman, K. F. (1983). Reaction time, intelligence and attention. Intelligence, 7, 329–44 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(83)90008-9].

Carmona da Mota, H., Antonio, A. M., Leitao, G., and Porto, M. (1990). Late effects of early malnutrition. The Lancet, 335, 1158 [DOI: 10.1016/0140-6736(90)91161-3].

Carpenter, P. A. and Just, M. A. (1975). Sentence comprehension: A psycholingusitic processing model of verification. Psychological Review, 82, 45–73 [DOI: 10.1037/h0076248].

Carpenter, P. A. and Just, M. A. (1986)(1986). Spatial ability: An information processing approach to psychometrics. In Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (vol. 3), (ed. R. J. Sternberg). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., and Schell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological Review, 97, 404–31 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.404].

Carraher, T. N., Carraher, D., and Schliemann, A. D. (1985). Mathematics in the streets and in the schools. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 21–9.

Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Carroll, J. B. (2003). The higher-stratum structure of human abilities: Current evidence supports g and about ten broad factors. In The scientific study of general intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen (ed. H. Nyborg). Oxford: Pergamon.

Caryl, P. G. (1994). Early event-related potentials correlate with inspection time and intelligence. Intelligence, 18, 15–46 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(94)90019-1].

Case, R., Demetriou, A., Platsidou, M., and Kazi, S. (2001). Integrating concepts and tests of intelligence from the differential and developmental traditions. Intelligence, 29, 307–36 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(00)00057-X].

Casey, M. B., Nuttall, R., Pezaris, E., and Benbow, C. P. (1995). The influence of spatial ability on gender differences in mathematics college entrance test scores across diverse samples. Developmental Psychology, 31, 697–705 [DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.31.4.697].

Casey, M. B., Nuttall, R. L., and Pezaris, E. (2001). Spatial-mechanical reasoning skills versus mathematical self-confidence as mediators of gender differences on mathematics subtests using cross-national gender-based items. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 32, 28–57 [DOI: 10.2307/749620].

Caspi, A., Williams, B., Kim-Cohen, J., et  al. (2007). Moderation of breastfeeding effects on the IQ by genetic variation in fatty acid metabolism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 18860–5 [DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0704292104].

Cattell, J. M. (1890). Mental tests and measurements. Mind, 15, 373–81.

Cattell, R. B. (1937). The fight for our national intelligence. London: King and Sons.

Cattell, R. B. (1940). A culture-free intelligence test, Part I. Journal of Educational Psychology, 31, 161–79 [DOI: 10.1037/h0059043].

Cattell, R. B. (1950). Personality. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth and action. Boston: Houghton-Miflin.

Ceci, S. J. (1991). How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its cognitive components? A re-assessment of the evidence. Developmental Psychology, 27, 703–22 [DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.5.703].

Ceci, S. J. (1996). On Intelligence  …. . .  more or less: A bio-ecological treatise on intellectual development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Ceci, S. J. and Liker, J. K. (1986). A day at the races; a study of IQ, expertise and cognitive complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 255–66 [DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.115.3.255].

Ceci, S. J. and Roazzi, A. (1994). The effects of context on cognition: postcards from Brazil. In Mind in context. Interactionist perspectives on human intelligence (eds R. J. Sternberg and R. K. Wagner). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ceci, S. J. and Williams, W. M. (eds) (2007). Why aren’t more women in science: Top researchers debate the evidence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Ceci, S. J., Barnett, S. M., and Kanaya, T. (2003). Developing childhood proclivities into adult competencies: The overlooked multiplier effect. In The psychology of abilities, competencies, and expertise (eds R. J. Sternberg and E. L. Grigorenko). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ceci, S. J., Williams, W. M., and Barnett, S. M. (2009). Women’s underrepresentation in science: Sociocultural and biological considerations. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 218–61 [DOI: 10.1037/a0014412] [PubMed: 19254079].

Cerella, J. (1985). Information processing rates in the elderly. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 67–83 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.98.1.67] [PubMed: 4034819].

Chaiken, S. R. (1993). Two models for an inspection time paradigm: Processing distraction and processing speed versus processing speed and asymptotic strength. Intelligence, 17, 257–83 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(93)90002-M].

Chaiken, S. R. (1994). The inspection time not studied: Processing speed ability unrelated to psychometric intelligence. Intelligence, 19, 295–316 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(94)90003-5].

Chakraborty, R., Kamboh, M. I., Nwankwo, M., and Ferrell, R. E. (1992). Caucasian genes in American blacks: New data. American Journal of Human Genetics, 50, 145–55 [PubMed: 1729885].

Chapin, F. S. (1928). A quantitative scale for rating the home and social environment of middle class families in an urban community. Journal of Educational Psychology, 19, 99–111 [DOI: 10.1037/h0074500].

Chase, W. G. and Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skill and working memory, In The psychology of learning and motivation (vol. 16), (ed. G. H. Bower). New York: Academic Press.

Chase, W. G. and Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55–81 [DOI: 10.1016/0010-0285(73)90004-2].

Cheng, P. and Holyoak, K. J. (1985). Pragmatic reasoning schemas. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 391–416 [DOI: 10.1016/0010-0285(85)90014-3] [PubMed: 4064622].

Cherrier, M. M., Asthana, S., Plymate, S., et  al. (2001). Testosterone supplementation improves spatial and verbal memory in healthy older men. Neurology, 57, 80–8 [PubMed: 11445632].

Chi, M. T. H. (1978). Knowledge structures and memory development. In Children’s thinking: What develops? (ed. R. S. Siegler). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Chipuer, H. M., Rovine, M. J., and Plomin, R. (1990). LISREL modeling: Genetic and environmental influences on IQ revisited. Intelligence, 14, 11–29 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(90)90011-H].

Choi, J. and Silverman, I. (2003). Processes underlying sex differences in route-learning strategies in children and adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 1153–66 [DOI: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00105-8].

Chomsky, N. (1972). Psychology and ideology. Cognition, 1, 11–46 [DOI: 10.1016/0010-0277(72)90043-1].

Chorney, M. J., Chorney, K., Seese, N., et  al. (1998). A quantitative trait locus associated with cognitive ability in children. Psychological Science, 9, 159–66 [DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.00032].

Cianciolo, A. T. and Sternberg, R. J. (2004). Intelligence: A brief history. Oxford: Blackwell.

Cianciolo, A. T., Grigorenko, E. L., Jarvin, L., et  al. (2006). Practical intelligence and tacit knowledge: Advancements in the measurement of developing expertise. Learning and Individual Differences, 16, 235–53 [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2006.04.002].

Clark, H. H. and Chase, W. G. (1972). On the process of comparing sentences against pictures. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 472–517 [DOI: 10.1016/0010-0285(72)90019-9].

Clayton, N. S. (2001). Hippocampal growth and maintenance depend on food-caching experience in juvenile mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli). Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 614–25 [DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.115.3.614] [PubMed: 11439450].

Cleeremans, A. and McClelland, J. L. (1991). Learning the structure of event sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 235–53 [DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.120.3.235].

Cleeremans, A., Destrebecqz, A., and Boyer, M. (1998). Implicit learning: News from the front. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 406–16 [DOI: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01232-7] [PubMed: 21227256].

Cochran, G., Hardy, J., and Harpending, H. (2006). Natural history of Ashkenazi intelligence. Journal of Biosocial Science, 38, 659–93 [DOI: 10.1017/S0021932005027069] [PubMed: 16867211].

Cohen, A. B. (2009). Many forms of culture. American Psychologist, 64, 194–204 [DOI: 10.1037/a0015308] [PubMed: 19348520].

Cohen, G. (1989). Memory in the real world. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Cohen, J. D., Forman, S. D., Braver, T. S., et  al. (1993). Activation of the prefrontal cortex in a nonspatial working memory task with functional MRI. Human Brain Mapping, 1, 293–304 [DOI: 10.1002/hbm.460010407].

Cole, M. (1971). The cultural context of learning and thinking: An exploration in experimental anthropology. New York: Basic Books.

Coleman, J. S. (1966). Equality of educational opportunity. Washington, DC: US Office of Education.

Collette, F., Van der Linden, M., Laureys, S., et  al. (2005). Exploring the unity and diversity of the neural substrates of executive functioning. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 409–23 [DOI: 10.1002/hbm.20118] [PubMed: 15852470].

Colom, R. and Abad, F. J. (2007). Advanced progressive matrices and sex differences: Comment to Mackintosh and Bennett 2005 (2005). Intelligence, 35, 183–5.

Colom, R. and Flores-Mendoza, C. E. (2007). Intelligence predicts scholastic achievement irrespective of SES factors: Evidence from Brazil. Intelligence, 35, 243–51 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.008].

Colom, R. and García-López, O. (2002). Sex differences in fluid intelligence among high school graduates. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 445–51 [DOI: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00040-X].

Colom, R. and Lynn, R. (2004). Testing the developmental theory of sex differences in intelligence on 12–18-year-olds. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 75–82 [DOI: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00053-9].

Colom, R., Abad, F. J., Quiroga, M. A., Shih, P. C., and Flores-Mendoza, C. (2008). Working memory and intelligence are highly related constructs, but why? Intelligence, 36, 584–606 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.01.002].

Colom, R., Haier, R. J., Head, K., et  al. (2009). Gray matter correlates of fluid, crystallized, and spatial intelligence: Testing the P-FIT model. Intelligence, 37, 124–35 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.07.007].

Conrad, R. (1979). The deaf schoolchild. London: Harper and Row.

Conti, G., Galeotti, A., Mueller, G., and Pudney, S. (2009). Popularity (ISER working papers 2009–03). Colchester, England: Institute for Social and Economic Research.

Conway, A. R. A. and Engle, R. W. (1994). Working memory and retrieval: A resource-dependent inhibition model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 354–73 [DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.123.4.354].

Conway, A. R. A. and Engle, R. W. (1996). Individual differences in working memory capacity: More evidence for a general capacity theory. Memory, 4, 577–90 [DOI: 10.1080/741940997] [PubMed: 8934455].

Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., and Bunting, M. F. (2001). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 331–5 [DOI: 10.3758/BF03196169].

Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., Bunting, M. F., Therriault, D. J., and Minkoff, S. R. B. (2002). A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 30, 163–84 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00096-4].

Conway, A. R. A., Getz, S. J., Macnamara, B., and Engel de Abreu, P. M. J. (2011). Working memory and intelligence. In Cambridge handbook of intelligence (eds R. J. Sternberg and S. B. Kaufman). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Conway, A. R. A., Jarrold, C., Kane, M., Miyake, A., and Towse, J. (eds) (2008). Variation in working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Cooper, H., Nye, B., Charlton, K., and Lindsay, J. (1996). The effects of summer vacation on achievement test scores: A narrative and meta-analytic review. Review of Educational Research, 66, 227–68 [DOI: 10.2307/1170523].

Cooper, L. A. (1975). Mental rotation of random two-dimensional shapes. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 20–43 [DOI: 10.1016/0010-0285(75)90003-1].

Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31, 187–276.

Cote, S. and Miners, C. T. H. (2006). Emotional intelligence, cognitive intelligence, and job performance. Administrative Science Quarterly, 51, 1–28.

Court, J. H. (1983). Sex differences in performance on Raven’s Progressive Matrices: A review. Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 29, 54–74.

Court, J. H. and Raven, J. (1995). Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. Section  7: Research and references. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.

Cowan, N. (1992). Verbal memory span and the timing of spoken recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 668–84 [DOI: 10.1016/0749-596X(92)90034-U].

Cowan, N., Elliott, E. M., Saults, J. S., et  al. (2005). On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes. Cognitive Psychology, 51, 42–100 [DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.12.001].

Cox, G. M. (1926). Genetic studies of genius (vol. 2). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Coyle, T. R. (2003). A review of the worst performance rule: Evidence, theory, and alternative hypotheses. Intelligence, 31, 567–87 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(03)00054-0].

Craik, F. I. M. and Salthouse, T. A. (eds) (2002). The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd edn). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Crawford, J. R., Deary, I. J., Allan, K. M., and Gustafsson, J.-E. (1998). Evaluating competing models of the relationship between inspection time and psychometric intelligence. Intelligence, 26, 27–42 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(99)80050-6].

Credé, M. and Kuncel, N. R. (2008). Study habits, skills, and attitudes: The third pillar supporting collegiate academic performance. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 425–53 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00089.x].

Crombie, I. K., Todman, J., McNeill, G., Florey, C. D., Menzies, I., and Kennedy, R. A. (1990). Effect of vitamin and mineral supplementation on verbal and non-verbal reasoning of school children. Lancet, 335, 744–7 [DOI: 10.1016/0140-6736(90)90867-5] [PubMed: 1969509].

Cudeck, R. (2000). Exploratory factor analysis. In Handbook of applied multivariate statistics and mathematical modeling (eds H. E. A. Tinsley and S. D. Brown). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Back to top

Dabbs, J. M. Jr, Chang, E. L., Strong, R. A., and Milun, R. (1998). Spatial ability, navigation strategy, and geographic knowledge among men and women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 89–98 [DOI: 10.1016/S1090-5138(97)00107-4].

Daley, T. C., Whaley, S. E., Sigman, M. D., Espinosa, M. P., and Neumann, C. (2003). IQ on the rise: The Flynn effect in rural Kenyan children. Psychological Science, 14, 215–19 [DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.02434] [PubMed: 12741743].

Daneman, M. and Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–66 [DOI: 10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6].

Daneman, M. and Green, I. (1986). Individual differences in comprehending and producing words in context. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 1–18 [DOI: 10.1016/0749-596X(86)90018-5].

Daneman, M. and Tardif, T. (1987). Working memory and reading skill re-examined. In Attention and performance XII (ed. M. Coltheart). Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Darity, W. A. Jr. and Mason, P. L. (1998). Evidence on discrimination in employment: codes of color, codes of gender. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 12, 63–90.

Dar-Nimrod, I. and Heine, S. J. (2006). Exposure to scientific theories affects women’s math performance. Science, 314, 435 [DOI: 10.1126/science.1131100] [PubMed: 17053140].

Davie, R., Butler, N., and Goldstein, H. (1972). From birth to seven: A report of the National Child Development Study. London: Longman.

Davis, O. S. P., Haworth, C. M. A., and Plomin, R. (2009). Dramatic increase in heritability of cognitive development from early to middle childhood: An 8-year longitudinal study of 8,700 pairs of twins. Psychological Science, 20, 1301–8 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02433.x] [PubMed: 19732386].

De Groot, A. D. (1951). War and the intelligence of youth. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 46, 596–7 [DOI: 10.1037/h0057014] [PubMed: 14880378].

De Groot, A. D. (1965). Thought and choice in chess. The Hague: Mouton.

de Houwer, J. (2009). The propositional approach to associative learning as an alternative for association formation models. Learning & Behavior, 37, 1–20.

de Jong, P. F. and Das Smaal, E. A. (1995). Attention and intelligence: The validity of the Star Counting Test. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 80–92 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.87.1.80].

Deary, I. J. (1993). Inspection time and WAIS-R IQ subtypes: A confirmatory factor analysis study. Intelligence, 17, 223–236 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(93)90029-5].

Deary, I. J. (1994). Intelligence and auditory discrimination: separating processing speed and fidelity of stimulus representation. Intelligence, 18, 189–213 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(94)90027-2].

Deary, I. J. (2000). Looking down on human intelligence: From psychometrics to the brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Deary, I. J. (2003). Reaction time and psychometric intelligence: Jensen’s contributions. In The scientific study of general intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen (ed. H.  Nyborg). Oxford: Pergamon.

Deary, I. J. and Caryl, P. G. (1993). Intelligence, EEG and evoked potentials. In Biological approaches to the study of human intelligence (ed. P. A. Vernon). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Deary, I. J. and Der, G. (2005). Reaction time explains IQ’s association with death. Psychological Science, 16, 64–9 [DOI: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00781.x] [PubMed: 15660853].

Deary, I. J., Allerhand, M., and Der, G. (2009). Smarter in middle age, faster in old age: A cross-lagged panel analysis of reaction time and cognitive ability over 13 years in the West of Scotland Twenty-07 Study. Psychology and Aging, 24, 40–7 [DOI: 10.1037/a0014442] [PubMed: 19290736].

Deary, I. J. Der, G., and Ford, G. (2001). Reaction times and intelligence differences: A population-based cohort study. Intelligence, 29, 389–99 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00062-9].

Deary, I. J., Irwing, P., Der, G., and Bates, T. (2007a). Brother-sister differences in the g factor in intelligence: Analysis of full, opposite-sex siblings from the NLSY1979. Intelligence, 35, 451–6 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.09.003].

Deary, I. J., Strand, S., Smith, P., and Fernandes, C. (2007b). Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence, 35, 13–21 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.02.001].

Deary, I. J., Thorpe, G., Wilson, V., Starr, J. M., and Whalley, L. J. (2003). Population sex differences in IQ at age 11: The Scottish mental survey 1932. Intelligence, 31, 533–42.

Deary, I. J., Whalley, L. J., Lemmon, H., Crawford, J. R., and Starr, J. M. (2000). The stability of individual differences in mental ability from childhood to old age: Follow-up of the 1932 Scottish Mental Survey. Intelligence, 28, 49–55 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(99)00031-8].

Deary, I. J., Whiteman, M. C., Starr, J. M., Whalley, L. J., and Fox, H. C. (2004). The impact of childhood intelligence on later life: Following up the Scottish Mental Surveys of 1932 and 1947. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 130–47 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.130] [PubMed: 14717632].

Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., and Kramer, J. H. (2001). The Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System: Technical manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Delis, D. C., Lansing, A., Houston, W. S., et  al. (2007). Creativity lost: The importance of testing higher-level executive functions in school-age children and adolescents. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 25, 29–40 [DOI: 10.1177/0734282906292403].

della Malva, C. L., Stuss, D. T., D’Alton, J., and Willmer, J. (1993). Capture errors and sequencing after frontal brain lesions. Neuropsychologia, 31, 363–72 [DOI: 10.1016/0028-3932(93)90160-2] [PubMed: 8502372].

Dempster, F. N. (1981). Memory span: Sources of individual and developmental differences. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 63–100 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.89.1.63].

Der, G., Batty, G. D., and Deary, I. J. (2006). Effect of breast feeding on intelligence in children: Prospective study, sibling pairs analysis, and meta-analysis. British Medical Journal, 333, 945–8 [DOI: 10.1136/bmj.38978.699583.55] [PubMed: 17020911].

DeShon, R. P., Chan, D., and Weissbein, D. A. (1995). Verbal overshadowing effects on Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices: Evidence for multidimensional performance determinants. Intelligence, 21, 135–55 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(95)90023-3].

D’Esposito, M., Cooney, J. W., Gazzaley, A., Gibbs, S. E. B., and Postle, B. R. (2006). Is the prefrontal cortex necessary for delay task performance? Evidence from lesion and fMRI data. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 248–60 [PubMed: 16573858].

Detterman, D. K. (1987). What does reaction time tell us about intelligence? In Speed of information-processing and intelligence (ed. P. A. Vernon). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Detterman, D. K. (2002). General intelligence: Cognitive and biological explanations. In The general factor of intelligence: How general is it? (eds R. J. Sternberg and E. L. Grigorenko). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Detterman, D. K. and Daniel, M. H. (1989). Correlations of mental tests with each other and with cognitive variables are highest for low IQ groups. Intelligence, 13, 349–59 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(89)80007-8].

Detterman, D. K. and Spry, K. M. (1988). Is it smart to play the horses? Comment on ‘A day at the races: A study of IQ, expertise, and cognitive complexity’ (S. J. Ceci and J. K. Liker, 1986). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 91–5.

Detterman, D. K., Gabriel, L. T., and Ruthsatz, J. M. (2000). Intelligence and mental retardation. In Handbook of intelligence (ed. R. J. Sternberg). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Devlin, B., Daniels, M., and Roeder, K. (1997). The heritability of IQ. Nature, 388, 468–71 [DOI: 10.1038/41319] [PubMed: 9242404].

Diamond, A. (1985). Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated by infants’ performance on A-not-B. Child Development, 56, 868–83 [DOI: 10.2307/1130099] [PubMed: 4042750].

Diamond, J. M. (1998). Guns, germs, and steel: The fates of human societies. London: Vantage.

Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., and Roberts, A. C. (1996). Primate analogue of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: Effects of excitotoxic lesions of the prefrontal cortex in the marmoset. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110, 872–86 [DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.110.5.872] [PubMed: 8918991].

Dickens, W. T. and Flynn, J. R. (2001). Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved. Psychological Review, 108, 346–69 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.346] [PubMed: 11381833].

Dickens, W. T. and Flynn, J. R. (2006). Black Americans reduce the racial IQ gap: Evidence from standardization samples. Psychological Science, 17, 913–20 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01802.x] [PubMed: 17100793].

Dickinson, A. (1980). Contemporary animal learning theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Dickstein, L. S. (1978). The effects of figure on syllogistic reasoning. Memory and Cognition, 6, 76–83 [DOI: 10.3758/BF03197431].

Doll, J. and Mayr, U. (1987). Intelligenz und Schachleistung—eine Untersuchung an Schachexperten. Psychologische Beitrage, 29, 270–89.

Doppelmayr, M., Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., et  al. (2005). Intelligence related differences in EEG-bandpower. Neuroscience Letters, 381, 309–13 [DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.02.037] [PubMed: 15896490].

Downey, D. B. (2001). Number of siblings and intellectual development: The resource dilution explanation. American Psychologist, 56, 497–504 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.6-7.497] [PubMed: 11413873].

Duckworth, A. L. and Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Psychological Science, 16, 939–44 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01641.x] [PubMed: 16313657].

Duckworth, A. L. and Seligman, M. E. P. (2006). Self-discipline gives girls the edge: Gender in self-discipline, grades, and achievement test scores. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 198–208 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.198].

Dumaret, A. and Stewart, J. T. (1985). IQ, scholastic performance and behaviour of sibs raised in contrasting environments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26, 553–80 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1985.tb01641.x].

Duncan, J., Burgess, P., and Emslie, H. (1995). Fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 33, 261–8 [DOI: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)00124-8] [PubMed: 7791994].

Duncan, J., Emslie, H., Williams, P., Johnson, R., and Freer, C. (1996). Intelligence and the frontal lobe: The organization of goal-directed behaviour. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 257–303 [DOI: 10.1006/cogp.1996.0008] [PubMed: 8660786].

Duncan, J., Seitz, R. J., Kolodny, J., et  al. (2000). A neural basis for general intelligence. Science, 289, 457–60 [DOI: 10.1126/science.289.5478.457] [PubMed: 10903207].

Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58 (whole number 270), 1–113 (originally published in German in 1935).

Duyme, M., Dumaret, A.-C., and Tomkiewicz, S. (1999). How can we boost IQs of ‘dull children’? A late adoption study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96, 8790–4 [DOI: 10.1073/pnas.96.15.8790].

Back to top

El Koussy, A. A. H. (1935). The visual perception of space. British Journal of Psychology, 20 (Monograph Supplement).

Elashoff, J. D. and Snow, R. E. (1971). ‘Pygmalion’ reconsidered. Worthington, OH: Charles A. Jones Publishing.

Ellis, L. and Walsh, A. (2003). Crime, delinquency and intelligence: A review of the worldwide literature. In The scientific study of general intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen (ed. H. Nyborg). Oxford: Pergamon.

Ellis, N. (1994). Vocabulary acquisition: The implicit ins and outs of explicit cognitive mediation. In Implicit and explicit learning of languages (ed. N. Ellis). London: Academic Press.

Elo, I. T. (2009). Social class differentials in health and mortality: Patterns and explanations in comparative perspective. Annual Review of Sociology, 35, 553–72 [DOI: 10.1146/annurev-soc-070308-115929].

Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., and Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–31 [DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.128.3.309].

Ericsson, K. A. (2006). The influence of experience and deliberate practice on the development of superior expert performance. In Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (eds K. A. Ericsson, N.  Charness, P. Feltovich, and R. R. Hoffman). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ericsson, K. A. and Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. American Psychologist, 49, 725–47 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.49.8.725].

Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Feltovich, P., and Hoffman, R. R. (eds) (2006). Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ertl, J., and Schafer, E. (1969). Brain response correlates of psychometric intelligence. Nature, 223, 421–2 [DOI: 10.1038/223421a0] [PubMed: 5823282].

Esposito, G., Kirkby, B. S., Van Horn, J. D., Ellmore, T. M., and Berman, K. F. (1999). Context-dependent, neural system-specific neurophysiological concomitants of ageing: Mapping PET correlates during cognitive activation. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 122, 963–79.

Evans, B. and Waites, B. (1981). IQ and mental testing: An unnatural science and its social history. London: Macmillan.

Evans, J. S. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255–78 [DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629] [PubMed: 18154502].

Eyferth, K. (1961). Leistungen verschiedener Gruppen von Besatzungskindern in Hamburg—Weschler Intelligenztest für kinder (HAWIK). Archiv für die Gesamte Psychologie, 113, 222–41.

Eysenck, H. J. (1939). Primary mental abilities. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 9, 260–5.

Eysenck, H. J. (1979). The structure and measurement of intelligence. Springer Verlag: New York.

Eysenck, H. J. (ed.) (1982). A model for intelligence. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The natural history of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Eysenck, H. J. and Schoenthaler, J. J. (1997). Raising IQ level by vitamin and mineral supplementation. In Intelligence, heredity, and environment (eds R. J. Sternberg and E. Grigorenko). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Back to top

Fagan, J. F. and Holland, C. R. (2002). Equal opportunity and racial differences in IQ. Intelligence, 30, 361–87 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00080-6].

Fagan, J. F., Holland, C. R., and Wheeler, K. (2007). The prediction, from infancy, of adult IQ and achievement. Intelligence, 35, 225–31 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.007].

Fagan, J. F. III (2000). A theory of intelligence as processing: Implications for society. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 6, 168–79 [DOI: 10.1037/1076-8971.6.1.168].

Fan, H., Jackson, T., Yang, X., Tang, W., and Zhang, J. (2010). The factor structure of the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso emotional intelligence test V 2.0 (MSCEIT): A meta-analytic structural equation modeling approach. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 781–5.

Fancher, R. E. (1985). The intelligence men: Makers of the IQ controversy. New York: Norton.

Farah, M. J., Hammon, K. M., Levine, D. N., and Calvanio, R. (1988). Visual and spatial mental imagery: Dissociable systems of representation. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 439–62 [DOI: 10.1016/0010-0285(88)90012-6].

Farber, S. L. (1981). Identical twins reared apart: A reanalysis. New York: Basic Books.

Feingold, A. (1988). Cognitive gender differences are disappearing. American Psychologist, 43, 95–103 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.43.2.95].

Feingold, A. (1992). Sex differences in variability in intellectual abilities: A new look at an old controversy. Review of Educational Research, 62, 61–84 [DOI: 10.3102/00346543062001061].

Feldman, J., Kerr, B., and Streissguth, A. P. (1995). Correlational analyses of procedural and declarative learning performance. Intelligence, 20, 87–114 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(95)90007-1].

Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick,,, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., and Pethnick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, Serial No. 242.

Ferguson, R. F. (1998). Teachers’ perceptions and expectations and the black-white test score gap. In The black-white test score gap (eds C. Jencks and M. Phillips). Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.

Fick, M. L. (1929). Intelligence test results of poor white, native (Zulu), coloured and Indian school children and the educational and social implications. South African Journal of Science, 26, 904–20.

Finger, S. (1994). Origins of neuroscience: A history of explorations into brain functions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Fischer, C. S., Hout, M., Jankowski, M. S., et  al. (1996). Inequality by design: Cracking the bell curve myth. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Flashman, L. A., Andreasen, N. C., Flaum, M., and Swayze, V. W. II (1997). Intelligence and regional brain volumes in normal controls. Intelligence, 25, 149–60 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90039-8].

Fletcher, R. (1991). Science, ideology and the media: The Cyril Burt scandal. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.

Floud, J. and Halsey, A. H. (1957). Intelligence tests, social class and selection for secondary schools. British Journal of Sociology, 8, 33–9 [DOI: 10.2307/587403].

Flynn, J. R. (1980). Race, IQ and Jensen. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

Flynn, J. R. (1984). The mean IQ of Americans: Massive gains 1932 to 1978. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 29–51 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.95.1.29].

Flynn, J. R. (1987). Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 171–91 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.171].

Flynn, J. R. (1992). Asian Americans: Achievement beyond IQ. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Flynn, J. R. (1996). Group differences: Is the good society impossible? Journal of Biosocial Science, 28, 573–85 [PubMed: 8973010].

Flynn, J. R. (2000). IQ gains, WISC subtests, and fluid g: g theory and the relevance of Spearman’s hypothesis to race. In The nature of intelligence. (Novartis Foundation Symposium 233); (eds G. R. Bock, J. A. Goode, and K.  Webb). New York: Wiley.

Flynn, J. R. (2007). What is intelligence? Beyond the Flynn effect. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Flynn, J. R. (2008). Where have all the liberals gone? Race, class, and ideals in America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ford, M. E. and Tisak, M. S. (1983). A further search for social intelligence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 197–206.

Fox, M. C., Roring, R. W., and Mitchum, A. L. (2009). Reversing the speed-IQ correlation: Intra-individual variability and attentional control in the inspection time paradigm. Intelligence, 37, 76–80 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.08.002].

Frearson, W. M. and Eysenck, H. J. (1986). Intelligence, reaction time (RT) and a new ‘odd-man-out’ RT paradigm. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 807–17 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(86)90079-6].

Frearson, W. M., Barrett, P., and Eysenck, H. J. (1988). Intelligence, reaction time, and the effects of smoking. Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 497–519 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(88)90149-3].

Freeman, F. N., Holzinger, K. J., and Mitchell, B. C. (1928). The influence of environment on the intelligence, school achievement, and conduct of foster children. 27th Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education, 27, Part I.

Freeney, Y. and O’Connell, M. (2010). Wait for it: Delay-discounting and academic performance among an Irish adolescent sample. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 231–6 [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.12.009].

Friedman, L. (1995). The space factor in mathematics: Gender differences. Review of Educational Research, 65, 22–50 [DOI: 10.3102/00346543065001022].

Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., et  al. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science, 17, 172–9 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01681.x] [PubMed: 16466426].

Fry, A. F. and Hale, S. (1996). Processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence: Evidence for a developmental cascade. Psychological Science, 7, 237–41 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00366.x].

Frydman, M. and Lynn, R. (1989). The intelligence of Korean children adopted in Belgium. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 1323–5 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(89)90246-8].

Furlow, F. B., Armijo-Prewitt, T., Gangestad, S. W., and Thornhill, R. (1997). Fluctuating asymmetry and psychometric intelligence. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B : Biological Sciences, 264, 823–9 [DOI: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0115] [PubMed: 9265189].

Back to top

Galea, L. A. M. and Kimura, D. (1993). Sex differences in route-learning. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 53–65 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(93)90174-2].

Galea, L. A. M., Kavaliers, M., Ossenkopp, K. P., and Innes, D., Hargreaves, EL (1994). Sexually dimorphic spatial learning varies seasonally in two populations of deer mice. Brain Research, 635, 18–26 [DOI: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)91419-2] [PubMed: 8173954].

Galea, S., Sisco, S., and Vlahov, D. (2005). Reducing disparities in vaccination rates between different racial/ethnic and socioeconomic groups: the potential of community-based multilevel interventions. Journal of Ambulatory Care Management, 28, 49–51 [PubMed: 15682961].

Gallagher, A. M. and Kaufman, J. C. (2005). Gender differences in mathematics: What we know and what we need to know. In Gender differences in mathematics: An integrative psychological approach (eds A. M. Gallagher and J. C. Kaufman). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Gallagher, A. M. and Kaufman, J. C. (eds) (2005). Gender differences in mathematics: An integrative psychological approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Galton, F. (1869). Hereditary genius: An inquiry into its laws and consequences. London: MacMillan.

Galton, F. (1883). Inquiries into human faculty, and its development. London: MacMillan.

Galton, F. (1908). Memories of my life. London: Methuen.

Garber, H. L. (1988). The Milwaukee Project: Preventing mental retardation in children at risk. Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Retardation.

Garces, E., Thomas, D., and Currie, J. (2002). Longer term effects of Head Start. American Economic Review, 92, 999–1012 [DOI: 10.1257/00028280260344560].

Gardner, H. (1993). Frames of mind (2nd edn). New York: Basic Books.

Gardner, H. (1997). Extraordinary minds. New York: Basic Books.

Gardner, H. (2006). Multiple intelligences: New horizons (revised edn). New York: Basic Books.

Gathercole, S. E., and Baddeley, A. D. (1990). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition. A study of young children learning new names. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 439–54.

Gathercole, S. E., Adams, A.-M., and Hitch, G. J. (1994). Do young children rehearse? An individual-differences analysis. Memory & Cognition, 22, 201–7.

Gaulin, S. J. C. and Fitzgerald, R. W. (1989). Sexual selection for spatial-learning ability. Animal Behaviour, 37, 322–31 [DOI: 10.1016/0003-3472(89)90121-8].

Gaw, F. (1925). A study of performance tests. British Journal of Psychology, 15, 374–92.

Gazzaley, A. and D’Esposito, M. (2007). Unifying prefrontal cortex function: Executive control, neural networks, and top-down modulation. In The human frontal lobes: Functions and disorders (2nd edn), (eds B. L. Miller and J. L. Cummings). New York: Guilford Press.

Geary, D. C. (1998). Male, female: The evolution of human sex differences. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Gebauer, G. F. (2003). Implicit learning and intelligence. Unpublished Ph.D.Thesis, University of Cambridge.

Gebauer, G. F. and Mackintosh, N. J. (2007). Psychometric intelligence dissociates implicit and explicit learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 34–54 [DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.1.34].

Geisinger, K. F., Spies, R. A., Carlson, J. F., and Plake, B. S. (eds) (2007). The seventeenth mental measurements yearbook. Lincoln, NE: Buros Institute of Mental Measurements.

Geschwind, N. and Galaburda, A. M. (1987). Cerebral lateralization: Biological mechanisms, associations and pathology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Ghiselli, E. E. (1966). The validity of occupational aptitude tests. New York: Wiley.

Ghiselli, E. E. (1973). The validity of aptitude tests in personnel selection. Personnel Psychology, 26, 461–77 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.1973.tb01150.x].

Giannitrapini, D. (1985). The electrophysiology of intellectual function. Basel: Karger.

Gibbs, C. H. (2000). The life of Schubert. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Gick, M. L. and Holyoak, K. J. (1980). Analogical problem solving. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 306–55 [DOI: 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90013-4].

Gick, M. L. and Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 1–38 [DOI: 10.1016/0010-0285(83)90002-6].

Gignac, G., Vernon, P. A. and Wickett, J. C. (2003). Factors influencing the relationship between brain size and intelligence. In The scientific study of general intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen (ed. H. Nyborg). Oxford: Pergamon.

Gilhooly, K. J., Logie, R. H., Wetherick, N. E., and Wynn, V. (1993). Working memory and strategies in syllogistic reasoning tasks. Memory and Cognition, 21, 115–24 [DOI: 10.3758/BF03211170].

Gillham, N. W. (2001). A life of Sir Francis Galton: From African exploration to the birth of eugenics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Gittings, R. (1968). John Keats. London: Heinemann.

Glanzer, M. and Cunitz, A. R. (1966). Two storage mechanisms in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 351–60 [DOI: 10.1016/S0022-5371(66)80044-0].

Gobet, F. and Charness, N. (2006). Chess and games. In Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (eds K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. Feltovich, and R. R. Hoffman). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Goddard, H. H. (1911). Two thousand normal children measured by the Binet measuring scale of intelligence. Pedagogical Seminary, 18, 232–59.

Goddard, H. H. (1912). The Kallikak family: A study in the heredity of feeble-mindedness. New York: MacMillan.

Goddard, H. H. (1914). Feeble-mindedness: Its causes and consequences. New York: MacMillan.

Goddard, H. H. (1916). Publication of the Vineland Training School. No. 11. Vineland: N J..

Goddard, H. H. (1917). Mental tests and the immigrant. Journal of Delinquency, 2, 243–77.

Goel, V., Buchel, C., Frith, C., and Dolan, R. J. (2000). Dissociation of mechanisms underlying syllogistic reasoning. NeuroImage, 12, 504–14 [DOI: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0636] [PubMed: 11034858].

Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., et  al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101, 8174–9 [DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101].

Goldberg, R. A., Schwartz, S., and Stewart, M. (1977). Individual differences in cognitive processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 9–14 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.69.1.9] [PubMed: 864084].

Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam.

Goleman, D. (1998). What makes a leader? Harvard Business Review, Nov–Dec, 93–102 [PubMed: 10187249].

Gordon, H. (1923). Mental and scholastic tests among retarded children. London: Board of Education pamphlet no. 44.

Gordon, R. A. (1997). Everyday life as an intelligence test: Effects of intelligence and intelligence context. Intelligence, 24, 203–320 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90017-9].

Goswami, U. C. (1992). Analogical reasoning in children. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Gottesman, I. I. (1991). Schizophrenia genesis. New York: Freeman.

Gottfredson, L. S. (2002). g: Highly general and highly practical. In The general factor of intelligence: How general is it? (eds R. J. Sternberg and E. L. Grigorenko). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Gottfredson, L. S. (2003a). g, jobs, and life. In The scientific study of general intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen (ed. H. Nyborg). Oxford: Pergamon.

Gottfredson, L. S. (2003b). Dissecting practical intelligence theory: Its claims and evidence. Intelligence, 31, 343–97 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00085-5].

Gottfredson, L. S. and Deary, I. J. (2004). Intelligence predicts health and longevity, but why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 1–4 [DOI: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301001.x].

Gould, S. J. (1986) The flamingo’s smile. London: Penguin Books.

Gould, S. J. (1997). The mismeasure of man (2nd edn). London: Penguin Books.

Grabner, R. H., Neubauer, A. C., and Stern, E. (2006). Superior performance and neural efficiency: The impact of intelligence and expertise. Brain Research Bulletin, 69, 422–39 [DOI: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.02.009] [PubMed: 16624674].

Grantham-McGregor, S., Ani, C., and Fernald, L. (2001). The role of nutrition in intellectual development. In Environmental effects on cognitive abilities (eds R. J. Sternberg and E. L. Grigorenko). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Gray, J. R., Chabris, C. F., and Braver, T. S. (2003). Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 316–22 [DOI: 10.1038/nn1014] [PubMed: 12592404].

Green, C. S. and Bavelier, D. (2003). Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature, 423, 534–7 [DOI: 10.1038/nature01647] [PubMed: 12774121].

Griffiths, P., Demellweek, C., Fay, N., Robinson, P., and Davidson, D. (2000). Wechsler subscale IQ and subtest profile in early treated phenylketonuria. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 82, 209–15 [DOI: 10.1136/adc.82.3.209] [PubMed: 10685922].

Grigorenko, E. L. (2000). Heritability and intelligence. In Handbook of intelligence (ed. R. J. Sternberg). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Grissmer, D., Flanagan, A., and Williamson, S. (1998). Why did the black-white score gap narrow in the 1970s and 1980s? In The black-white test score gap (eds C. Jencks and M. Phillips). Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.

Grudnik, J. L. and Kranzler, J. H. (2001). Meta-analysis of the relationship between intelligence and inspection time. Intelligence, 29, 523–35 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00078-2].

Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Guilford, J. P. (1982). Cognitive psychology’s ambiguities: Some suggested remedies. Psychological Review, 89, 48–59 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.89.1.48].

Guilford, J. P. (1988). Some changes in the structure-of-intellect model. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 48, 1–4 [DOI: 10.1177/001316448804800102].

Guilford, J. P., and Hoepfner, R. (1971). The analysis of intelligence. New York: McGraw-HillMcGraw- Hill.

Gur, R. C., Turetsky, B. I., Matsui, M., et  al. (1999). Sex differences in brain gray and white matter in healthy young adults: Correlations with cognitive performance. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 4065–72 [PubMed: 10234034].

Gustafsson, J.-E. . 1988)(1988). Hierarchical models of individual differences in cognitive abilities. In Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (vol. 4), (ed. R. J. Sternberg). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Gustafsson, J.-E. and Undheim, J. O. (1996). Individual differences in cognitive functions. In Handbook of educational psychology (eds D. C. Berliner and R. C. Calfee). New York: Macmillan.

Gutkin, T. B. and Reynolds, C. R. (1981). Factorial similarity of the WISC-R for White and Black children from the standardization sample. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 227–31 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.73.2.227].

Guttman, L. (1957). Empirical verification of the radex structure of mental abilities and personality traits. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 17, 391–407 [DOI: 10.1177/001316445701700306].

Back to top

Haavisto, M.-L. and Lehto, J. E. (2005). Fluid/spatial and crystallized intelligence in relation to domain-specific working memory: A latent-variable approach. Learning and Individual Differences, 15, 1–21 [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2004.04.002].

Haier, R. J., Colom, R., Schroeder, D. H., et  al. (2009). Gray matter and intelligence factors: Is there a neuro-g? Intelligence, 37, 136–44 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.10.011].

Haier, R. J., Jung, R. E., Yeo, R. A., Head, K., and Alkire, M. T. (2005). The neuroanatomy of general intelligence: sex matters. NeuroImage, 25, 320–7 [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.019] [PubMed: 15734366].

Haier, R. J., Siegel, B. V., MacLachlan, A., and Soderling, E. (1992). Regional glucose metabolic changes after learning a complex visuospatial/motor task: A positron emission tomographic study. Brain Research, 570, 134–43 [DOI: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90573-R] [PubMed: 1617405].

Hall, V. C. and Kaye, D. B. (1980). Early patterns of cognitive development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. Serial no. 184.

Halpern, D. F. (2000). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (3rd edn). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Halpern, D. F. and Collaer, M. L. (2005). Sex differences in visuospatial abilities: More than meets the eye. In The Cambridge handbook of visuospatial thinking (eds P. Shah and A. Miyake). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Halpern, D. F., Wai, J., and Saw, A. (2005). A psychobiosocial model: Why females are sometimes greater than and sometimes less than males in math achievement. In Gender differences in mathematics: An integrative psychological approach (eds A. M. Gallagher and J. C. Kaufman). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Hanley, J. R., Young. A. W., and Pearson, N. A. (1991). Impairment of the visuo-spatial scratch pad. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43A, 101–25.

Harden, K. P., Turkheimer, E., and Loehlin, J. C. (2007). Genotype by environment interaction in adolescents’ cognitive aptitude. Behavior Genetics, 37, 273–83 [DOI: 10.1007/s10519-006-9113-4] [PubMed: 16977503].

Harnqvist, K. (1968). Relative changes in intelligence from  3–18. I. Background and methodology. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 9, 50–64 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.1968.tb00518.x] [PubMed: 5662227].

Harrell, T. W. and Harrell, M. S. (1945). Army General Classification Test scores for civilian occupations. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 5, 229–39 [DOI: 10.1177/001316444500500303].

Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do. New York: Free Press.

Hart, B. and Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H Brookes.

Hartigan, J. and Wigdor, A. K. (1989). Fairness in employment testing: Validity generalization, minority issues, and the General Aptitude Test Battery. Washington, DC: National Academies PressNational Academy Press.

Haworth, C. M. A., Wright, M. J., Luciano, M., et  al. (2009). The heritability of general cognitive ability increases linearly from childhood to young adulthood. Molecular Pscyhiatry, Advance publication, 2 June 2009.

Hearnshaw, L. S. (1979). Cyril Burt: Psychologist. London: Hodder and Stoughton.

Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley.

Hebb, D. O. and Penfield, W. (1940). Human behaviour after extensive removal from the frontal lobes. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 44, 421–38.

Heckman, J. J. (2006). Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children. Science, 312, 1900–2 [DOI: 10.1126/science.1128898] [PubMed: 16809525].

Hedges, L. V. and Nowell, A. (1995). Sex differences in mental test scores, variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science, 269, 41–5 [DOI: 10.1126/science.7604277] [PubMed: 7604277].

Hedges, L. V. and Nowell, A. (1998). Black-white test score convergence since 1965. In The black-white test score gap (eds C. Jencks and M. Phillips). Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.

Hedlund, J., Wilt, J. M., Nebel, K. L., Ashford, S. J., and Sternberg, R. J. (2006). Assessing practical intelligence in business school admissions: A supplement to the graduate management admissions test. Learning and Individual Differences, 16, 101–27 [DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2005.07.005].

Hegarty, M. and Waller, D. A. (2005). Individual differences in spatial abilities. In The Cambridge handbook of visuospatial thinking (eds A. Miyake and P. Shah). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Hegarty, M., Montello, D. R., Richardson, A. E., Ishikawa, T., and Lovelace, K. (2006). Spatial abilities at different scales: Individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning. Intelligence, 34, 151–76 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2005.09.005].

Heil, M. and Jansen-Osmann, P. (2008). Sex differences in mental rotation with polygons of different complexity: Do men utilize holistic processes whereas women prefer piecemeal ones? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 683–9 [DOI: 10.1080/17470210701822967] [PubMed: 18421643].

Heine, S. J. (2008). Cultural psychology. New York: Norton.

Hendrickson, A. E (1982). The biological basis of intelligence. Part I: Theory. In A model for intelligence (ed. H. J. Eysenck). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Hendrickson, A. E. and Hendrickson, D. E. (1980). The biological basis for individual differences in intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 1, 3–33 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(80)90003-3].

Hendrickson, D. E. (1982). The biological basis of intelligence. Part II: Measurement. In A model for intelligence (ed. H. J. Eysenck). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Henson, R. (2001). Neural working memory. In Working memory in perspective (ed. J. Andrade). New York: Psychology Press.

Hermelin, B. and O’Connor, N. (1978). The idiot savant: flawed genius or clever Hans? Psychological Medicine, 13, 479–81 [DOI: 10.1017/S0033291700047905].

Herrnstein, R. J. and Murray, C. (1994). The bell curve: Intelligence and class structure in American life. New York: Free Press.

Heyes, C. M. (1998). Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 101–34 [PubMed: 10097012].

Hick, W. E. (1952). On the rate of gain of information. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 4, 11–26 [DOI: 10.1080/17470215208416600].

Hill, L., Chorney, M. J., Lubinski, D., Thompson, L. A., and Plomin, R. (2002). A quantitative trait locus not associated with cognitive ability in children: A failure to replicate. Psychological Science, 13, 561–2 [DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.00499] [PubMed: 12430843].

Hirschi, T. and Hindelang, M. J. (1977). Intelligence and delinquency: A revisionist review. American Sociological Review, 42, 571–87 [DOI: 10.2307/2094556] [PubMed: 900659].

Hitch, G. J. (1978). The role of short-term working memory in mental arithmetic. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 302–23 [DOI: 10.1016/0010-0285(78)90002-6].

Hitch, G. J., Towse, J. N., and Hutton, U. (2001). What limits children’s working memory span? Theoretical accounts and applications for scholastic development. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 184–98 [DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.184].

Holahan, C. K. and Sears, R. R. (1995). The gifted group in later maturity. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Horgan, D. D. and Morgan, D. (1990). Chess expertise in children. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 4, 109–28 [DOI: 10.1002/acp.2350040204].

Horn, J. L. (1985). Remodeling old models of intelligence. In Handbook of intelligence (ed. B. B. Wolman). New York: Wiley.

Horn, J. L. (1972). Intelligence: Why it grows, why it declines. In Human intelligence (ed. J. M. Hunt). Oxford: Transaction Books.

Horn, J. L. and Cattell, R. B. (1966). Refinement and test of the theory of fluid and crystallized general intelligences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 57, 253–70 [DOI: 10.1037/h0023816] [PubMed: 5918295].

Horn, J. L. and Knapp, J. R. (1973). On the subjective character of the empirical base of Guilford’s structure-of-intellect model. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 33–43 [DOI: 10.1037/h0034681].

Horn, J. M., Loehlin, J. C., and Willerman, L. (1982). Aspects of the inheritance of intellectual abilities. Behavior Genetics, 12, 479–516 [DOI: 10.1007/BF01073781] [PubMed: 7168736].

Horn, W. F. and Packard, T. (1985). Early identification of learning problems: a meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 597–607 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.77.5.597].

Howard, A. and Bray, D. W. (1988). Managerial lives in transition: Advancing age and changing times. New York: Guilford Press.

Howe, M. J. A. (1999). Genius explained. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Howe, M. J. A. and Smith, J. (1988). Calendar calculating in ‘idiots savants’: how do they do it? British Journal of Psychology, 79, 371–86 [PubMed: 3167499].

Huang, M.-H. and Hauser, R. M. (1998). Trends in black-white test score differentials: II. The WORDSUM Vocabulary Test. In The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures (ed. U. Neisser). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Hugdahl, K., Thomsen, T., and Ersland, L. (2006). Sex differences in visuo-spatial processing: An fMRI study of mental rotation. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1575–83 [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.026] [PubMed: 16678867].

Huizink, A. C. and Mulder, E. J. H. (2006). Maternal smoking, drinking or cannabis use during pregnancy and neurobehavioral and cognitive functioning in human offspring. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 24–41 [DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.005] [PubMed: 16095697].

Hull, C. L. (1928). Aptitude testing. New York: World Book Co.

Hulme, C., Thomson, N., Muir, C., and Lawrence, A. (1984). Speech rate and the development of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 241–53 [DOI: 10.1016/0022-0965(84)90124-3].

Humphrey, N. K. (1976). The social function of intellect. In Growing points in ethology (eds P. P. G. Bateson and R. A. Hinde). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Humphreys, L. G. and Lubinski, D. (1996). Assessing spatial visualization: An underappreciated ability for many school and work settings. In Intellectual talent: Psychometric and social issues (eds C. P. Benbow and D. Lubinski). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Hunt, E. B. (1978). Mechanics of verbal ability. Psycho-logical Review, 85, 109–30.

Hunt, E. B. (1980). Intelligence as an information-processing concept. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 449–74 [PubMed: 7437671].

Hunt, E. B. (1985). Verbal ability. In Human abilities: An information-processing approach (ed. R. J. Sternberg). New York: Freeman.

Hunt, E. B. (1987). The next word on verbal ability. In Speed of information-processing and intelligence (ed. P. A. Vernon). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Hunt, E. B. (1995). Will we be smart enough? A cognitive analysis of the coming workforce. New York: Russell Sage.

Hunt, E. B. and Carlson, J. S. (2007). The standards for conducting research on topics of immediate social relevance. Intelligence, 35, 393–9 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.10.002].

Hunt, E. B., Lunneborg, C. L. and Lewis, J. (1975). What does it mean to be high verbal? Cognitive Psychology, 7, 194–227 [DOI: 10.1016/0010-0285(75)90010-9].

Hunt, T. (1928). The measurement of social intelligence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 12, 317–34 [DOI: 10.1037/h0075832].

Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., and Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236–48 [DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236].

Hyde, G. and Trickey, G. (1995). Differential Aptitude Test (DAT) for guidance—manual. London: Psychological Corporation.

Hyde, J. S. (1981). How large are cognitive gender differences? American Psychologist, 36, 892–901 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.36.8.892].

Hyde, J. S. (2007). Women in science: Gender similarities in abilities and sociocultural forces. In Why aren’t more women in science: Top researchers debate the evidence (eds S. J. Ceci and W. M. Williams). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Hyde, J. S. and Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 153–69.

Hyde, J. S., Fennema, E., and Lamon, S. J. (1990). Gender differences in mathematics performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 139–55 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.139] [PubMed: 2138794].

Back to top

Ilai, D. and Willerman, L. (1989). Sex differences in WAIS-R item performance. Intelligence, 13, 225–34 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(89)90019-6].

Inhelder, B. and Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence: An essay on the construction of formal operational structures. New York: Basic Books.

Irwin, R. J. (1984). Inspection time and its relation to intelligence. Intelligence, 8, 47–65 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(84)90006-0].

Irwing, P. and Lynn, R. (2005). Sex differences in means and variability on the progressive matrices in university students: A meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychology, 96, 505–24 [DOI: 10.1348/000712605X53542] [PubMed: 16248939].

Isgor, C. and Sengelaub, D. R. (1998). Prenatal gonadal steroids affect adult spatial behavior, CA1 and CA3 pyramidal cell morphology in rats. Hormones and Behavior, 34, 183–98 [DOI: 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1477] [PubMed: 9799628].

Back to top

Jackson, M. (1980). Further evidence for a relationship between memory access and reading ability. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 683–94 [DOI: 10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90367-9].

Jacobsen, C. F. (1936). Studies of cerebral function in primates. I. The functions of the frontal association areas in monkeys. Comparative Psychology Monographs, 13(3), 1–60.

Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513–41 [DOI: 10.1016/0749-596X(91)90025-F].

Jacoby, L. L., Debner, J. A., and Hay, J. F. (2001). Proactive interference, accessibility bias, and process dissociations: Valid subject reports of memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 686–700 [DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.27.3.686].

Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., and Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 6829–33 [DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0801268105].

Janssen, R., De Boeck, P., and Steene, G. V. (1996). Verbal fluency and verbal comprehension abilities in synonym tasks. Intelligence, 22, 291–310 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(96)90024-0].

Jencks, C. (1972). Inequality: A reassessment of the effect of family and schooling in America. New York: Basic Books.

Jencks, C. (1992). Rethinking social policy: Race, poverty, and the underclass. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Jencks, C. and Phillips, M. (eds) (1998). The black-white test score gap. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.

Jensen, A. R. (1969). How much can we boost IQ and scholastic achievement? Harvard Educational Review, 39, 1–123.

Jensen, A. R. (1980). Bias in mental testing. London: Methuen.

Jensen, A. R. (1987). Individual differences in the Hick paradigm. In Speed of information-processing and intelligence (ed. P. A. Vernon). Northwood, NJ: Ablex.

Jensen, A. R. (1989). Raising IQ without increasing g? A review of the Milwaukee Project: Preventing mental retardation in children at risk. Developmental Review, 9, 234–58 [DOI: 10.1016/0273-2297(89)90030-0].

Jensen, A. R. (1993). Spearman’s hypothesis tested with chronometric information-processing tasks. Intelligence, 17, 47–77 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(93)90039-8].

Jensen, A. R. (1997). The puzzle of nongenetic variance. In Intelligence, heredity and environment (eds R. J. Sternberg and E. Grigorenko). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.

Jensen, A. R. (2005). Mental chronometry and the unification of differential psychology. In Cognition and intelligence: Identifying the mechanisms of the mind (eds R. J. Sternberg and J. E. Pretz). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Jensen, A. R. (2006). Clocking the mind: Mental chronometry and individual differences. Oxford: Elsevier.

Jensen, A. R. and Figueroa, R. A. (1975). Forward and backward digit-span interaction with race and IQ: Predictions from Jensen’s theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 882–93 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.67.6.882] [PubMed: 1194502].

Jensen, A. R. and Reynolds, C. R. (1983). Sex differences on the WISC-R. Personality and Individual Differences, 4, 223–6 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(83)90029-6].

Jensen, A. R. and Sinha, S. N. (1993). Physical correlates of human intelligence. In Biological approaches to the study of human intelligence (ed. P. A. Vernon). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Jensen, A. R. and Whang, P. A. (1993). Reaction times and intelligence: A comparison of Chinese-American and Anglo-American children. Journal of Biosocial Science, 25, 397–410 [PubMed: 8360233].

Johns, M., Schmader, T., and Martens, A. (2005). Knowing is half the battle: teaching stereotype threat as a means of improving women’s math performance. Psychological Science, 16, 175–9 [DOI: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00799.x] [PubMed: 15733195].

Johnson, M. H. and Morton, J. (1991). Biology and cognitive development. Oxford: Blackwell.

Johnson, W. and Bouchard, T. J. Jr. (2005). The structure of human intelligence: It is verbal, perceptual, and image rotation (VPR), not fluid and crystallized. Intelligence, 33, 393–416 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2004.12.002].

Johnson, W. and Bouchard, T. J. Jr. (2007). Sex differences in mental abilities: g masks the dimensions on which they lie. Intelligence, 35, 23–39 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.03.012].

Johnson, W., Bouchard, T. J. Jr, Krueger, R. F., McGue, M., and Gottesman, I. I. (2004). Just one g: Consistent results from three test batteries. Intelligence, 32, 95–107 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(03)00062-X].

Johnson, W., Brett, C. E., and Deary, I. J. (2010). Intergenerational class mobility in Britain: A comparative look across three generations in the Lothian birth cohort 1936. Intelligence, 38, 268–81.

Johnson, W., te Nijenhuis, J., and Bouchard, T. J. Jr. (2008). Still just 1 g: Consistent results from five test batteries. Intelligence, 36, 81 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2007.06.001].

Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2001). Mental models and deduction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 434–42 [DOI: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01751-4] [PubMed: 11707382].

Johnson-Laird, P. N. and Byrne, R. (1991). Deduction. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Jones, C. M., Braithwaite, V. A., and Healy, S. D. (2003). The evolution of sex differences in spatial ability. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 403–11 [DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.3.403] [PubMed: 12802870].

Jorde, L. B. and Wooding, S. P. (2004). Genetic variation, classification and ‘race’. Nature Genetics, 36, S28–33 [DOI: 10.1038/ng1435] [PubMed: 15508000].

Joynson, R. B. (1989). The Burt affair. London: Routledge.

Juel-Nielsen, N. (1980). Individual and environment: Monozygotic twins reared apart. New York; International Universities Press.

Juhel, J. (1993). Should we take the shape of reaction time distributions into account when studying the relationship between RT and psychometric intelligence? Personality and Individual Differences, 15, 357–60 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(93)90231-Q].

Jung, R. E. and Haier, R. J. (2007). The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 135–54 [DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X07001185] [PubMed: 17655784].

Jurden, F. H. (1995). Individual differences in working memory and complex cognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 93–102 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.87.1.93].

Back to top

Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58, 697–720 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697] [PubMed: 14584987].

Kail, R. V. (1988). Developmental functions for speeds of cognitive processes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 45, 339–364 [DOI: 10.1016/0022-0965(88)90036-7] [PubMed: 3385355].

Kail, R. V. (1991). Processing time declines exponentially during childhood and adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 27, 259–66 [DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.259].

Kail, R. V. (2007). Longitudinal evidence that increases in processing speed and working memory enhance children’s reasoning. Psychological Science, 18, 312–3 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01895.x] [PubMed: 17470254].

Kail, R. V. and Salthouse, T. A. (1994). Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychologica, 86, 199–225 [DOI: 10.1016/0001-6918(94)90003-5] [PubMed: 7976467].

Kamin, L. J. (1974). The science and politics of IQ. Potomac, Md.MD: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Kamin, L. J. (1981). Intelligence: The battle for the mind. (H. J. Eysenck versus Leon Kamin). London: Macmillan.

Kandel, E., Mednick, S. A., Kirkegaard-Sorensen, L., et  al. (1988). IQ as a protective factor for subjects at high risk for antisocial behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 224–6 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.2.224] [PubMed: 3372829].

Kane, M. J. (2005). Full frontal fluidity? Looking in on the neuroimaging of reasoning and intelligence. In Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence (eds O. Wilhelm and R. W. Engle). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Kane, M. J. and Engle, R. W. (2000). Working-memory capacity, proactive interference, and divided attention: Limits on long-term memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 336–58 [DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.2.336].

Kane, M. J. and Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 47–70 [DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.47].

Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., and Conway, A. R. A. (2005). Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence are strongly related constructs: Comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle 2005 (2005). Psychological Bulletin, 131, 66–71.

Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., Tuholski, S. W., et  al. (2004). The generality of working memory capacity: A latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 189–217 [DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.189].

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 389–98 [DOI: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01230-3] [PubMed: 21227254].

Kaufman, A. S. (2001a). Do low levels of lead produce IQ loss in children? A careful examination of the literature. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, 303–41 [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/16.4.303].

Kaufman, A. S. (2001b). How dangerous are low (not moderate or high) doses of lead for children’s intellectual development? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, 403–31 [DOI: 10.1093/arclin/16.4.403].

Kaufman, A. S. and Doppelt, J. E. (1976). Analysis of WISC-R standardization data in terms of the stratification data. Child Development, 47, 165–71 [DOI: 10.2307/1128295].

Kaufman, S. B. (2007). Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial visualization ability: Can they be accounted for by differences in working memory capacity? Intelligence, 35, 211–23 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.009].

Kaufman, S. B. (2011). Intelligence and the cognitive unconscious. In Cambridge handbook of intelligence (eds R. J. Sternberg and S. B. Kaufman). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., Brown, J., and Mackintosh, N. (2009). Associative learning predicts intelligence above and beyond working memory and processing speed. Intelligence, 37, 374–82 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2009.03.004].

Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., et  al. (2010). Implicit learning as an ability. Cognition, 116, 321–40 [DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.011] [PubMed: 20573341].

Kavšek, M. (2004). Predicting later IQ from infant visual habitation and dishabituation: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 25, 369–93 [DOI: 10.1016/j.appdev.2004.04.006].

Keating, D. P. (1978). A search for social intelligence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 218–23 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.70.2.218].

Keith, T. Z., Reynolds, M. R., Patel, P. G., and Ridley, K. P. (2008). Sex differences in latent cognitive abilities ages  6 to 59: Evidence from the Woodcock-Johnson III tests of cognitive abilities. Intelligence, 36, 502–25 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2007.11.001].

Keren, G. and Schul, Y. (2009). Two is not always better than one: A critical evaluation of two-system theories. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 533–50 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01164.x].

Kerr, R., Garvin, J., Heaton, N., and Boyle, E. (2006). Emotional intelligence and leadership effectiveness. Leadership and Organization Development Journal, 27, 265–79 [DOI: 10.1108/01437730610666028].

Kevles, D. J. (1985). In the name of eugenics: Genetics and the uses of human heredity. New York: Knopf.

Klemp, G. O. and McClelland, D. C. (1986). What characterizes intelligent functioning among senior managers? In Practical intelligence: Nature and origins of competence in the everyday world (eds R. J. Sternberg and R. K. Wagner). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Kline, P. (1994). An easy guide to factor analysis. London: Routledge.

Knudsen, E. I., Heckman, J. J., Cameron, J. L., and Shonkoff, J. P. (2006). Economic, neurobiological, and behavioral perspectives on building America’s future workforce. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 10155–62 [DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0600888103].

Kondrashov, A. S. (1995). Contamination of the genome by very slightly deleterious mutations: Why have we not died 100 times over? Journal of Theoretical Biology, 175, 583 [DOI: 10.1006/jtbi.1995.0167] [PubMed: 7475094].

Kovács, K. (2010). A component process account of the general factor of intelligence. Unpublished Ph.D. Thesis, University of Cambridge.

Kranzler, J. H. (1992). A test of Larson and Alderton’s (1990) worst performance rule of reaction time variability. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 255–61 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(92)90099-B].

Kranzler, J. H. and Jensen, A. R. (1991). The nature of psychometric g: Unitary process or a number of independent processes? Intelligence, 15, 397–422 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(91)90003-V].

Krashen, S. D. (1989). We acquire vocabulary and spelling by reading: Additional evidence for the input hypothesis. The Modern Language Journal, 73, 440–64 [DOI: 10.2307/326879].

Kristensen, P. and Bjerkedal, T. (2007). Explaining the relation between birth order and intelligence. Science, 316, 1717 [DOI: 10.1126/science.1141493] [PubMed: 17588924].

Kyllonen, P. C. and Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working memory capacity?! Intelligence, 14, 389–433 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(05)80012-1].

Kyllonen, P. C., Lohman, D. F., and Snow, R. E. (1984). Effects of aptitudes, strategy training, and task facets on spatial task performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 130–45 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.76.1.130].

Back to top

La Pointe, L. B. and Engle, R. W. (1990). Simple and complex word spans as measures of working memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 1118–33 [DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.16.6.1118].

La Voie, D. and Light, L. L. (1994). Adult age differences in repetition priming: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 9, 539–53 [DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.9.4.539] [PubMed: 7893425].

Lansdown, R. and Yule, W. (eds) (1986). The lead debate: The environment, toxicology, and child health. London: Croom Helm.

Langley, C. M. (1994). Spatial memory in the desert kangaroo rat (Dipodomys deserti). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 3–14 [DOI: 10.1037/0735-7036.108.1.3] [PubMed: 8174344].

Langsford, P. B., Mackenzie, B. D., and Maher, D. P. (1994). Auditory inspection time, sustained attention, and the fundamentality of mental speed. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 487–97 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(94)90074-4].

Lansman, M., Donaldson, G., Hunt, E., and Yantis, S. (1982). Ability factors and cognitive processes. Intelligence, 6, 347–86 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(82)90024-1].

Lareau, A. (2003). Unequal childhoods: Class, race, and family life. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Larson, G. E. and Alderton, D. L. (1990). Reaction time variability and intelligence: Worst performance’analysis of individual differences. Intelligence, 14, 309–25.

Larson, G. E. and Saccuzzo, D. P. (1989). Cognitive correlates of general intelligence: Toward a process theory of g. Intelligence, 13, 5–31 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(89)90003-2].

Larson, G. E., Merritt, C. R., and Williams, S. E. (1988). Information processing and intelligence: Some implications of task complexity. Intelligence, 12, 131–47 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(88)90012-8].

Lasley, S. M., Green, M. C., and Gilbert, M. E. (1999). Influence of exposure period on in vivo hippocampal glutamate and GABA release in rats chronically exposed to lead. Neurotoxicology, 20, 619–30 [PubMed: 10499360].

Lave, J., Murtaugh, M., and de la Roche, O. (1984). The dialectic of arithmetic in grocery shopping. In Everyday cognition: Its development in social context (eds B. Rogoff and J. Lave). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Lavin, D. E. (1965). The prediction of academic performance: A theoretical analysis and review of research. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

Lawrence, E. M. (1931). An investigation into the relation between intelligence and inheritance. British Journal of Psychology (Monograph Supplement), 16, 1–80.

Lazar, I. and Darlington, R. (1982). Lasting effects of early education: A report from the consortium for longitudinal studies. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 47.

Leahy, A. (1935). Nature-nurture and intelligence. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 17, 236–308.

Leather, C. V. and Henry, L. A. (1994). Working memory span and phonological awareness tasks as predictors of early reading ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 88–111 [DOI: 10.1006/jecp.1994.1027] [PubMed: 8064220].

Lee, K. H., Choi, Y. Y., Gray, J. R., et  al. (2006). Neural correlates of superior intelligence: Stronger recruitment of posterior parietal cortex. NeuroImage, 29, 578–86 [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.036] [PubMed: 16122946].

Legree, P. J., Pifer, M. E., and Grafton, F. C. (1996). Correlations among cognitive abilities are lower for higher ability groups. Intelligence, 23, 45–57 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(96)80005-5].

Levin, S. L., Mohamed, F. B., and Platek, S. M. (2005). Common ground for spatial cognition? A behavioral and fMRI study of sex differences in mental rotation and spatial working memory. Evolutionary Psychology, 3, 227–54.

Levy, J. (1976). Cerebral lateralization and spatial ability. Behavior Genetics, 6, 171–88 [DOI: 10.1007/BF01067147] [PubMed: 1267738].

Lewin, R. and Foley, R. A. (2004). Principles of human evolution (2nd edn). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Lewis, E. O. (1933). Types of mental deficiency and their social significance. Journal of Mental Science, 79, 293–304.

Li, S.-C., Lindenberger, U., Hommel, B., et  al. (2004). Transformations in the couplings among intellectual abilities and constituent cognitive processes across the life span. Psychological Science, 15, 155–63 [DOI: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503003.x] [PubMed: 15016286].

Linn, M. C. and Peterson, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479–98 [DOI: 10.2307/1130467].

Locke, E. A. (2005). Why emotional intelligence is an invalid concept. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 425–31 [DOI: 10.1002/job.318].

Locurto, C. (1991). Beyond IQ in preschool programs? Intelligence, 15, 295–312 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(91)90038-F].

Loehlin, J. C. (1989). Partitioning environmental and genetic contributions to behavioral development. American Psychologist, 44, 1285–92 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.10.1285] [PubMed: 2679255].

Loehlin, J. C. and Nichols, R. C. (1976). Heredity, environment and personality: A study of 850 sets of twins. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.

Loehlin, J. C., Horn, J. M., and Willerman, L. (1989). Modeling IQ change: Evidence from the Texas adoption project. Child Development, 60, 993–1004 [DOI: 10.2307/1131039] [PubMed: 2758892].

Loehlin, J. C., Vandenberg, S. G., and Osborne, R. T. (1973). Blood group genes and Negro-white ability differences. Behavior Genetics, 3, 263–70 [DOI: 10.1007/BF01067603] [PubMed: 4750245].

Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuo-spatial working memory. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Logie, R. H. and Marchetti, C. (1991). Visuo-spatial working memory: Visual, spatial or central executive? In Mental images in human cognition (eds R. H. Logie and M. Denis). Amsterdam: North Holland Press.

Logie, R. H., Gilhooly, K. J., and Wynn, V. (1994). Counting on working memory in arithmetic problem solving. Memory and Cognition, 22, 395–410 [DOI: 10.3758/BF03200866].

Lohman, D. F. (1986). The effect of speed-accuracy trade-off on sex differences in mental rotation. Perception and Psychophysics, 39, 427–36 [DOI: 10.3758/BF03207071] [PubMed: 3748756].

Lohman, D. F. . 1988)(1988). Spatial abilities as traits, processes, and knowledge. In Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (vol. 4), (ed. R. J. Sternberg). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Lohman, D. F. (2000). Complex information processing and intelligence. In Handbook of intelligence (ed. R. J. Sternberg). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Lubinski, D. (2004). Introduction to the special section on cognitive abilities: 100 years after Spearman’s (1904) “General intelligence, objectively determined and measured”. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 96–111 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.96] [PubMed: 14717630].

Lubinski, D. and Benbow, C. P. (2006). Study of mathematically precocious youth after 35 years: Uncovering antecedents for the development of math-science expertise. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 316–45 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00019.x].

Lubinski, D. and Humphreys, L. G. (1990). A broadly based analysis of mathematical giftedness. Intelligence, 14, 327–55 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(90)90022-L].

Lubinski, D. and Humphreys, L. G. (1992). Some bodily and medical correlates of mathematical giftedness and commensurate levels of socioeconomic status. Intelligence, 16, 99–115 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(92)90027-O].

Lucas, A., Morley, R., Cole, T. J., Lister, G., and Leeson-Payne, C. (1992). Breast milk and subsequent intelligence quotient in children born preterm. The Lancet, 339, 261–4 [DOI: 10.1016/0140-6736(92)91329-7].

Luders, E., Gaser, C., Narr, K. L., and Toga, A. W. (2009a). Why sex matters: Brain size independent differences in gray matter distributions between men and women. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 14265–70 [DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2261-09.2009].

Luders, E., Narr, K. L., Thompson, P. M., et  al. (2009b). Gender differences in cortical complexity. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 799–800 [DOI: 10.1038/nn1277].

Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. London: Tavistock Publications.

Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive development, its cultural and social foundations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Lynn, R. (1982). IQ in Japan and the United States shows a growing disparity. Nature, 297, 222–3 [DOI: 10.1038/297222a0].

Lynn, R. (1992). Does Spearman’s g decline at high-IQ levels? Some evidence from Scotland. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 153, 229–30 [PubMed: 1512590].

Lynn, R. (1994). Sex differences in intelligence and brain size: A paradox resolved. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 257–71 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(94)90030-2].

Lynn, R. (1996a). Dysgenics: Genetic deterioration in modern populations. Westport, CT: Praeger.

Lynn, R. (1996b). Racial and ethnic differences in intelligence in the United States on the Differential Ability Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 271–3 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(95)00158-1].

Lynn, R. (1998). In support of the nutrition theory. In The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures (ed. U. Neisser). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Lynn, R. (2006). Race differences in intelligence: An evolutionary analysis. Augusta, GA: Washington Summit.

Lynn, R. (2008). What has caused the Flynn effect? Secular increases in the Development Quotients of infants. Intelligence, 37, 16–24 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.07.008].

Lynn, R. (2009). Fluid intelligence but not vocabulary has increased in Britain, 1979–2008. Intelligence, 37, 249–55 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.09.007].

Lynn, R. and Hampson, S. (1986). The rise of national intelligence: evidence from Britain, Japan and the USA. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 23–32 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(86)90104-2].

Lynn, R. and Irwing, P. (2004). Sex differences on the progressive matrices: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 32, 481–98 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2004.06.008].

Lynn, R. and Longley, D. (2006). On the high intelligence and cognitive achievements of Jews in Britain. Intelligence, 34, 541–7 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.03.011].

Lynn, R., Allik, J., and Irwing, P. (2004). Sex differences on three factors identified in Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices. Intelligence, 32, 411–24 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2004.06.007].

Lynn, R., Hampson, S. L., and Howden, V. (1988). The intelligence of Scottish children, 1932–1986. Studies in Education, 6, 19–25.

Lynn, R., Irwing, P., and Cammock, T. (2002). Sex differences in general knowledge. Intelligence, 30, 27–39 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00064-2].

Lynn, R., Wilson, R. G., and Gault, A. (1989). Simple musical tests as measures of Spearman’s g. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 25–8 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(89)90173-6].

Back to top

Maccoby, E. E. and Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Mackenzie, B. D. and Bingham, E. (1985). IQ, inspection time, and response strategies in a university population. Australian Journal of Psychology, 37, 257–68 [DOI: 10.1080/00049538508256403].

MacKinnon, D. W. (1962). The nature and nurture of creative talent. American Psychologist, 17, 484–95 [DOI: 10.1037/h0046541].

Mackintosh, N. J. (1998). IQ and human intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Mackintosh, N. J. (2007). Reply to Colom and Abad 2006 (2006). Intelligence, 35, 301–2.

Mackintosh, N. J. (ed.) (1995). Cyril Burt: Fraud or framed? Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Mackintosh, N. J. and Bennett, E. S. (2002). IT, IQ and perceptual speed. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 685–93 [DOI: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00069-1].

Mackintosh, N. J. and Bennett, E. S. (2003). The fractionation of working memory maps onto different components of intelligence. Intelligence, 31, 519–31 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(03)00052-7].

Mackintosh, N. J. and Bennett, E. S. (2005). What do Raven’s Matrices measure? An analysis in terms of sex differences. Intelligence, 33, 663–74 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2005.03.004].

Mackintosh, N. J. and Mascie-Taylor, C. G. N. (1986). The IQ question. In Personality, cognition and values (eds C. Bagley and G. K. Verma). Macmillan: London.

MacLeod, C. M., Hunt, E. B., and Mathews, N. N. (1978). Individual differences in the verification of sentence-picture relationships. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 493–507 [DOI: 10.1016/S0022-5371(78)90293-1].

Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., et  al. (2000). Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 4398–403 [DOI: 10.1073/pnas.070039597].

Mahaffey, K. R., Annest, J. L., Roberts, J., and Murphy, R. S. (1982). National estimates of blood lead levels: United States, 1976–1980. New England Journal of Medicine, 307, 573–9 [DOI: 10.1056/NEJM198209023071001] [PubMed: 7110203].

Maher, J. and Macfarlane, A. (2004). Inequalities in infant mortality: trends by social class, registration, mother’s age and birthweight, England and Wales, 1976–2000. Health Statistics Quarterly, 24, 14–22 [PubMed: 15704808].

Manktelow, K. I. and Over, D. E. (1990). Deontic thought and the selection task. In Lines of thinking: Reflections on the psychology of thinking (vol. 1); (eds K. Gilhooly, M. Keane, R. Logie, and G. Erdos). Chichester: Wiley.

Manning, W. H. and Jackson, R. (1984). College entrance examinations: Objective selection or gatekeeping for the economically privileged. In Perspectives on bias in mental testing (eds C. R. Reynolds and R. T. Brown). New York: Plenum.

Manolakes, L. A. (1997). Cognitive ability, environmental factors, and crime: Predicting frequent criminal activity. In Intelligence, genes, and success: Scientists respond to the bell curve (eds B. Devlin, S. E. Fienberg and K. Roeder). New York: Springer.

Marjoribanks, K. (1972). Ethnic and environmental influences on mental abilities. American Journal of Sociology, 78, 323–37 [DOI: 10.1086/225327].

Marlowe, H. A. (1986). Social intelligence: Evidence for multidimensionality and construct independence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 52–8 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.78.1.52].

Martorell, R. (1998). Nutrition and the worldwide rise in IQ scores. In The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures (ed. U. Neisser). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Mascie-Taylor, C. G. N. (1984). Biosocial correlates of IQ.  In The biology of human intelligence (eds C. J. Turner and H. B. Miles);. London: Proceedings of the 20th Annual Symposium of the Eugenics Society.

Mascie-Taylor, C. G. N. and Gibson, J. B. (1978). Social mobility and IQ components. Journal of Biosocial Science, 10, 263–76 [PubMed: 690162].

Masse, L. N. and Barnett, W. S. (2002). A benefit cost analysis of the Abecedarian Early Childhood Intervention. New Brunswick, NJ: National Institute for Early Education Research.

Masters, M. S. and Sanders, B. (1993). Is the gender difference in mental rotation disappearing? Behavior Genetics, 23, 337–41 [DOI: 10.1007/BF01067434] [PubMed: 8240213].

Masunaga, H. and Horn, J. (2001). Expertise and age-related changes in components of intelligence. Psychology and Aging, 16, 293–311 [DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.2.293] [PubMed: 11405317].

Matthews, G., Zeidner, M., and Roberts, R. D. (2005). Emotional intelligence: An elusive ability? In Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence (eds O. Wilhelm and R. W. Engle). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Matthews, M. H. (1987). Sex differences in spatial competence: The ability of young children to map ‘primed’ unfamiliar environments. Educational Psychology, 7, 77–90 [DOI: 10.1080/0144341870070201].

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., and Caruso, D. R. (2002). Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT), . Version 2.0. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., and Caruso, D. R. (2004). Emotional intelligence: Theory, findings, and implications. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 197–215 [DOI: 10.1207/s15327965pli1503_02].

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., and Caruso, D. R. (2008). Emotional intelligence: New ability or eclectic traits? American Psychologist, 63, 503–17 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.63.6.503] [PubMed: 18793038].

Mayes, L. C. and Fahy, T. (2001). Prenatal drug exposure and cognitive development. In Environmental effects on cognitive abilities (eds R. J. Sternberg and E. L. Grigorenko). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Mazzocco, M. M. M. and Reiss, A. L. (1999). Normal variation in size of the FMR2 gene is not associated with variation in intellectual performance. Intelligence, 27, 175–82 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(99)00019-7].

McCall, R. B. (1977). Childhood IQ’s as predictors of adult educational and occupational status. Science, 197, 482–3 [DOI: 10.1126/science.197.4302.482] [PubMed: 17783247].

McCall, R. B. and Carriger, M. S. (1993). A meta-analysis of infant habituation and recognition memory performance as predictors of later IQ. Child Development, 64, 57–79 [DOI: 10.2307/1131437] [PubMed: 8436038].

McCarthy, R. A. and Warrington, E. K. (1990). Cognitive neuropsychology. London: Academic Press.

McCartney, M., Harris, O., and Bernieri, F. (1990). Growing up and growing apart: A developmental meta-analysis of twin studies. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 226–37 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.226] [PubMed: 2138795].

McClelland, D. C. (1973). Testing for competence rather than for ‘intelligence’. American Psychologist, 28, 1–14 [DOI: 10.1037/h0034092] [PubMed: 4684069].

McCrae, R. R. and Costa, P. T. Jr. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509–16 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.5.509] [PubMed: 9145021].

McDaniel, M. A. (2005). Big-brained people are smarter: A meta-analysis of the relationship between in vivo brain volume and intelligence. Intelligence, 33, 337–46 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2004.11.005].

McGarry-Roberts, P. A., Stelmack, R. M., and Campbell, K. B. (1992). Intelligence, reaction time, and event-related potentials. Intelligence, 16, 289–313 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(92)90011-F].

McGeorge, P., Crawford, J. R., and Kelly, S. W. (1996). The relationship between WAIS-R abilities and speed of processing in a word identification task. Intelligence, 23, 175–90 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(96)90002-1].

McGrew, K. S. (2009). CHC theory and the human cognitive abilities project: Standing on the shoulders of the giants of psychometric intelligence research. Intelligence, 37, 1–10 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.08.004].

McGue, M., Bouchard, T. J. Jr. Iacono, W. G., and Lykken, D. T. (1993). Behavioral genetics of cognitive ability: A life-span perspective. In Nature, nurture and psychology (eds R. Plomin and G. E. McClearn). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

McLaren, I. P. L., Green, R. E. A., and Mackintosh, N. J. (1994). Animal learning and the implicit/explicit distinction. In Implicit and explicit learning of languages (ed. N. C. Ellis). London: Academic Press.

McLoughlin, C. S. and Koh, T. H. (1982). Testing intelligence: A decision suitable for the psychologist. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 35, 308–11.

McMichael, A. J., Baghurst, P. A., Wigg, N. R., Vimpani, V. G., Robertson, E. F., and Roberts, R. J. (1988). Port Pirie cohort study: Environmental exposure to lead and children’s abilities at the age of four years. New England Journal of Medicine, 319, 468–75 [DOI: 10.1056/NEJM198808253190803] [PubMed: 3405253].

Medawar, P. B. (1982). Pluto’s republic. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Melnick, M., Myrianthopoulos, N. C., and Christian, J. C. (1978). The effects of chorion type on variation in IQ in the NCPP twin population. American Journal of Human Genetics, 30, 425–33 [PubMed: 568880].

Mercer, J. R. (1973). Labeling the retarded. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Mercer, J. R. and Lewis, J. F. (1978). System of multicultural pluralistic assessment. New York: Psychological Corporation.

Merkel, J. (1885). Die zeitlichen Verhaltnisse der Willensthatigkeit. Philosophische Studien, 2, 73–127.

Merton, R. K. (1968). Social theory and social structure. New York: Free Press.

Meyers, C. E., Nihira, K., and Zetlin, A. (1979). The measurement of adaptive behavior. In The handbook of mental deficiency, psychological theory and research (2nd edn), (ed. N. Ellis). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Michael, J. S. (1988). A new look at Morton’s craniological research. Current Anthropology, 29, 349–54 [DOI: 10.1086/203646].

Miller, G. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. London: Heineman.

Milner, B. (1963). Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting: the role of the frontal lobes. Archives of Neurology, 9, 90–100.

Mingroni, M. A. (2007). Resolving the IQ paradox: Heterosis as a cause of the Flynn effect and other trends. Psychological Review, 114, 806–29 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.3.806] [PubMed: 17638507].

Miyake, A. and Shah, P. (eds) (1999). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Moffat, S. D., Hampson, E., and Hatzipantelis, M. (1998). Navigation in a ‘virtual’ maze: Sex differences and correlation with psychometric measures of spatial ability in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 73–87 [DOI: 10.1016/S1090-5138(97)00104-9].

Moody, D. E. (2009). Can intelligence be increased by training on a task of working memory? Intelligence, 37, 327–8 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2009.04.005].

Moore, D. S. and Johnson, S. P. (2008). Mental rotation in human infants: A sex difference. Psychological Science, 19, 1063–6 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02200.x] [PubMed: 19076473].

Moore, E. G. J. (1986). Family socialization and the IQ test performance of traditionally and transracially adopted black children. Developmental Psychology, 22, 317–26 [DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.22.3.317].

Morley, R. (1996). The influence of early diet on later development. Journal of Biosocial Science, 28, 481–7 [PubMed: 8973005].

Morris, G. L. and Alcorn, M. B. (1995). Raven’s progressive matrices and inspection time: P200 slope correlates. Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 81–7 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(94)00126-D].

Morris, R. G. M. (1981). Spatial localization does not require the presence of local cues. Learning and Motivation, 12, 239–60 [DOI: 10.1016/0023-9690(81)90020-5].

Mumaw, R. J. and Pellegrino, J. W. (1984). Individual differences in complex spatial processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 920–39 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.76.5.920].

Mumaw, R. J., Pellegrino, J. W., Kail, R. V., and Carter, P. (1984). Different slopes for different folks: Process analysis of spatial aptitude. Memory and Cognition, 12, 515–21 [DOI: 10.3758/BF03198314].

Murdoch, S. (2007). IQ: The brilliant idea that failed. London: Duckworth.

Murray, C. (1998). Income, inequality, and IQ. Washington, DC: American Enterprise Institute.

Back to top

Nagoshi, C. T. and Johnson, R. A. (1987). Cognitive ability profiles of Caucasian vs. Japanese subjects in the Hawaii Family Study of Cognition. Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 581–3 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(87)90223-6].

Neisser, U. (1976). General, academic, and artificial intelligence. In The nature of intelligence (ed. L. Resnick). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Neisser, U. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51, 77–101 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77].

Neisser, U. (ed.) (1998). The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures. Washington: American Psychological Association.

Nettelbeck, T. (1987). Inspection time and intelligence. In Speed of information processing and intelligence (ed. P.  A. Vernon). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Nettelbeck, T. (2003). Inspection time and g. In The scientific study of general intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen (ed. H. Nyborg). Oxford: Pergamon.

Nettelbeck, T. and Burns, N. R. (2010). Processing speed, working memory and reasoning ability from childhood to old age. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 379–84 [DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.10.032].

Nettelbeck, T. and Kirby, N. H. (1983). Measures of timed performance and intelligence. Intelligence, 7, 39–52 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(83)90005-3].

Nettelbeck, T. and Lalley, M. (1976). Inspection time and measured intelligence. British Journal of Psychology, 67, 17–22.

Nettelbeck, T. and Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1992). Aging, cognitive performance, and mental speed. Intelligence, 16, 189–205 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(92)90004-B].

Nettelbeck, T., Edwards, C., and Vreugdenhil, A. (1986). Inspection time and IQ: Evidence for a mental speed-ability association. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 633–41 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(86)90032-2].

Nettle, D. (2007). Empathizing and systemizing: What are they, and what do they contribute to our understanding of psychological sex differences? British Journal of Psychology, 98, 237–55 [DOI: 10.1348/000712606X117612] [PubMed: 17456271].

Neubauer, A. C. and Fink, A. (2009). Intelligence and neural efficiency: Measures of brain activation versus measures of functional connectivity in the brain. Intelligence, 37, 223–9 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.10.008].

Neubauer, A. C. and Freudenthaler, H. H. (1994). Reaction times in a sentence-picture verification test and intelligence: Individual strategies and effects of extended practice. Intelligence, 19, 193–218 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(94)90013-2].

Neubauer, A. C., Fink, A. and Schrausser, D. G. (2002). Intelligence and neural efficiency: The influence of task content and sex on the brain-IQ relationship. Intelligence, 30, 515–36.

Newcombe, N. S. (2007). Taking science seriously: Straight thinking about spatial sex differences. In Why aren’t more women in science: Top researchers debate the evidence (eds S. J. Ceci and W. M. Williams). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Newman, D. L., Tellegen, A., and Bouchard, T. J. Jr. (1998). Individual differences in adult ego development: Sources of influence in twins reared apart. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 985–95 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.4.985] [PubMed: 9569654].

Newman, H. H., Freeman, F. N., and Holzinger, K. H. (1937). Twins: A study of heredity and environment. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Newman, S. D., Carpenter, P. A., Varma, S., and Just, M. A. (2003). Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1668–82 [DOI: 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00091-5] [PubMed: 12887991].

Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently  …. . .  and why. New York: Free Press.

Nisbett, R. E. (2005). Heredity, environment, and race differences in IQ: A commentary on Rushton and Jensen 2005 (2005). Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 11, 302–10.

Nisbett, R. E. (2009). Intelligence and how to get it: Why schools and cultures count. New York: Norton.

Norman, D. A. and Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behaviour. In Consciousness and self-regulation. Advances in research and theory (vol. 4.), (eds R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwarts, and D. Shapiro). New York: Plenum Press.

Nuttall, R., Casey, M. B., and Pezaris, E. (2005). Spatial ability as a mediator of gender differences on mathematics tests: A biological-environmental framework. In Gender differences in mathematics: An integrative psychological approach (eds A. M. Gallagher and J. C. Kaufman). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Back to top

Oberauer, K., Schulze, R., Wilhelm, O., and Suss, H.-M. (2005). Working memory and intelligence—their correlation and their relation: Comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle 2005 (2005). Psychological Bulletin, 131, 61–5.

O’Connor, N., Cowan, R., and Samella, K. (2000). Calendrical calculation and intelligence. Intelligence, 28, 31–48 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(99)00028-8].

O’Connor, T. A. and Burns, N. R. (2003). Inspection time and general speed of processing. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 713–24 [DOI: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00264-7].

Oden, M. H. (1968). The fulfillment of promise: 40-year follow-up of the Terman gifted group. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 77, 3–93 [PubMed: 5640199].

Ones, D. S., Viswesvaran, C., and Dilchert, S. (2005). Cognitive ability in selection decisions. In Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence (eds O. Wilhelm and R. W. Engle). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Oswald, W. D. and Roth, E. (1978). Der Zahlen-Verbindungs-Test (ZVT). Gottingen, Germany: Hogrefe.

O’Toole, B. I. (1990). Intelligence and behaviour and motor vehicle accident mortality. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 22, 211–21 [DOI: 10.1016/0001-4575(90)90013-B] [PubMed: 2393469].

O’Toole, B. I. and Stankov, L. (1992). Ultimate validity of psychological tests. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 699–716 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(92)90241-G].

Owen, A. M., Downes, J. J., Sahakian, B. J., Polkey, C. E., and Robbins, T. W. (1990). Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 28, 1021–34 [DOI: 10.1016/0028-3932(90)90137-D] [PubMed: 2267054].

Back to top

Page, E. B., and Grandon, G. M. (1981). Massive intervention and child intelligence: The Milwaukee Project in critical perspective. Journal of Special Education, 15, 239–56 [DOI: 10.1177/002246698101500211].

Palmer, B. R., Gignac, G., Manocha, R., and Stough, C. (2005). A psychometric evaluation of the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test, Version 2.0. Intelligence, 33, 285–305.

Park, G., Lubinski, D., and Benbow, C. P. (2008). Ability differences among people who have commensurate degrees matter for scientific creativity. Psychological Science, 19, 957–61 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02182.x] [PubMed: 19000201].

Pedersen, N. L., Plomin, R., Nesselroade, J. R., and McClearn, G. E. (1992). A quantitative genetic analysis of cognitive abilities during the second half of the life span. Psychological Science, 3, 346–53 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00045.x].

Pellegrino, J. W. (1986). Deductive reasoning ability. In Human abilities: An information-processing approach (ed. R. J. Sternberg). New York: W. H. Freeman.

Penn, D. C. and Povinelli, D. J. (2007). On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a ‘theory of mind’. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362, 731–44 [DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2006.2023].

Pennington, B. F., Filipek, P. A., Lefly, D., et  al. (2000). A twin MRI study of size variations in the human brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 223–32 [DOI: 10.1162/089892900561850] [PubMed: 10769318].

Penrose, L. S. and Raven, J. C. (1936). A new series of perceptual tests: Preliminary communication. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 16, 97–104.

Perruchet, P. (1994). Learning from complex rule-governed environments: On the proper functions of nonconscious and conscious processes. In Attention and  performance, XV (eds C. Umilta and M.  Moscovitch). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Petrides, K. V., Furnham, A., and Frederickson, N. (2004). Emotional intelligence. The Psychologist, 17, 574–7.

Phillip, E. E. (1973). Discussion in Law and ethics of AID. and embryo transfer, Ciba Symposium (eds G. E. W. Wolstenholme and D. W. Fitzsimons). Amsterdam: North-Holland/Elsevier.

Phillips, M., Brooks-Gunn, J., Duncan, G. J., Klebanov, P., and Crane, J. (1998). Family background, parenting practices, and the black-white test score gap. In The black-white test score gap (eds C. Jencks and M. Phillips). Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.

Piaget, J. and Inhelder, B. (1969). The psychology of the child. New York: Basic Books.

Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G. E., and McGuffin, P. (2008). Behavioral genetics (5th edn). New York: Worth.

Plomin, R., Fulker, D. W., Corley, R., and DeFries, J. C. (1997). Nature, nurture and cognitive development from 1 to 16 years: A parent-offspring adoption study. Psychological Science, 8, 442–7 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00458.x].

Plomin, R., McClearn, G. E., Pedersen, N. L., Nesselroade, J. R., and Bergeman, C. S. (1988). Genetic influence on childhood family environment perceived retrospectively from the last half of the life span. Developmental Psychology, 24, 738–45 [DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.24.5.738].

Pollack R. H. and Brenner, M. W. (eds) (1969) The experimental psychology of Alfred Binet; Selected papers. New York: Springer.

Poltrock, S. E. and Agnoli, F. .(1986). Are spatial visualization ability and visual imagery ability equivalent? In Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (vol. 3), (ed. R. J. Sternberg). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Porteus, S. D. (1931). The psychology of a primitive people: A study of the Australian aborigine. Oxford: Longmans.

Posthuma, D., De Geus, E. J. C., Baare, W. F. C., et  al. (2002). The association between brain volume and intelligence is of genetic origin. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 83–4 [DOI: 10.1038/nn0202-83] [PubMed: 11818967].

Prabhakaran, V. and Rypma, B. (2007). P-FIT and the neuroscience of intelligence: How well does P fit? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 166–7 [DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X07001355].

Prabhakaran, V., Smith, J. A. L., Desmond, J. E., and Glover, G. H., Gabrieli, JD (1997). Neural substrates of fluid reasoning: An fMRI study of neocortical activation during performance of the Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 43–63 [DOI: 10.1006/cogp.1997.0659] [PubMed: 9212721].

Puts, D. A., McDaniel, M. A., Jordan, C. L., and Breedlove, S. M. (2008). Spatial ability and prenatal androgens: Meta-analyses of congenital adrenal hyperplasia and digit ratio (2D: :4D) studies. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 100–11 [DOI: 10.1007/s10508-007-9271-3] [PubMed: 18074217].

Back to top

Quinn, P. C. and Liben, L. S. (2008). A sex difference in mental rotation in young infants. Psychological Science, 19, 1067–70 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02201.x] [PubMed: 19076474].

Back to top

Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1993). Crystal quest: A search for the basis of maintenance of practised skills into old age. In Attention: Selection, awareness, and control: A tribute to Donald Broadbent (eds A. D. Baddeley and L. Weiskrantz). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Rabbitt, P. M. A., Scott, M., Lunn, M., et  al. (2007). White matter lesions account for all age-related declines in speed but not in intelligence. Neuropsychology, 21, 363–70 [DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.3.363] [PubMed: 17484599].

Ramey, C. T. (1992). High-risk children and IQ: Altering intergenerational patterns. Intelligence, 16, 239–56 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(92)90007-E].

Ramey, C. T. (1993). A rejoinder to Spitz’s critique of the Abecedarian experiment. Intelligence, 17, 337–45.

Ramey, C. T., Ramey, S. L., and Lanzi, R. G. (2001). Intelligence and experience. In Environmental effects on cognitive abilities (eds R. J. Sternberg and E. L. Grigorenko). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Raven, J. (2000). The Raven’s Progressive Matrices: Change and stability over culture and time. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 1–48 [DOI: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0735] [PubMed: 10945921].

Raven, J. C. (1938). Progressive Matrices: A perceptual test of intelligence. London: H. K. Lewis.

Raz, N. (2000). Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance: Integration of structural and functional findings. In The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd edn), (eds F. I. M. Craik and T. A. Salthouse). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 317–27 [DOI: 10.1016/S0022-5371(67)80119-1].

Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Reber, A. S., Walkenfeld, F. F., and Hernstadt, R. (1991). Implicit and explicit learning: Individual differences and IQ. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17, 888–96 [DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.17.5.888].

Reber, P. J., Gitelman, D. R., Parrish, T. B., and Mesulam, M. M. (2003). Dissociating explicit and implicit category knowledge with fMRI. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 574–83 [DOI: 10.1162/089892903321662958] [PubMed: 12803968].

Record, R. G., McKeown, T., and Edwards, J. H. (1969). The relation of measured intelligence to birth order and maternal age. Annals of Human Genetics, 33, 61–−9 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-1809.1969.tb01630.x] [PubMed: 5821320].

Ree, M. J., Carretta, T. R., and Green, M. T. (2003). The ubiquitous role of g in training. In The scientific study of general intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen (ed. H. Nyborg). Oxford: Pergamon.

Reichle, E. D., Carpenter, P. A., and Just, M. A. (2000). The neural bases of strategy and skill in sentence-picture verification. Cognitive Psychology, 40, 261–95 [DOI: 10.1006/cogp.2000.0733] [PubMed: 10888341].

Reiss, D., Neiderhiser, J. M., Hetherington, E. M., and Plomin, R. (2000). The relationship code: Deciphering genetic and social influences on adolescent development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Renner, J. M. and Rosenzweig, M. R. (1987). Enriched and impoverished environments: Effects on brain and behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Reschly, D. J. and Sabers, D. L. (1979). An examination of bias in predicting MAT scores from WISC-R scores for four ethnic-racial groups. Journal of Educational Measurement, 16, 1–9 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1745-3984.1979.tb00080.x].

Rescorla, R. A. (1988). Pavlovian conditioning: It’s not what you think it is. American Psychologist, 43, 151–60 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.43.3.151] [PubMed: 3364852].

Retherford, R. D. and Sewell, W. H. (1988). Intelligence and family size reconsidered. Social Biology, 35, 1–40 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1988.tb00454.x] [PubMed: 3217809].

Retherford, R. D. and Sewell, W. H. (1991). Birth order and intelligence: further tests of the confluence model. American Sociological Review, 56, 141–58 [DOI: 10.2307/2095775].

Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2002). New visions of the aging mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 394–400 [DOI: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01957-5] [PubMed: 12200182].

Reynolds, C. R., Chastain, R. L., Kaufman, A. S., and McLean, J. E. (1987). Demographic characteristics and IQ among adults: Analysis of the WAIS-R standardization sample as a function of the stratification variables. Journal of School Psychology, 25, 323–42 [DOI: 10.1016/0022-4405(87)90035-5].

Reynolds, M. R. and Keith, T. Z. (2007). Spearman’s law of diminishing returns in hierarchical models of intelligence for children and adolescents. Intelligence, 35, 267–81 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.08.002].

Rhodes, L., Dustman, R., and Beck, E. (1969). The visual evoked response: A comparison of bright and dull children. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 27, 364–72 [DOI: 10.1016/0013-4694(69)91445-X] [PubMed: 4186734].

Rips, L. J. (1994). The psychology of proof: Deductive reasoning in human thinking. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Robb, G. (2000). Rimbaud. New York: Norton.

Roberts, R. D. and Stankov, L. (1999). Individual differences in speed of mental processing and human cognitive abilities: Toward a taxonomic model. Learning and Individual Differences, 11, 1–120 [DOI: 10.1016/S1041-6080(00)80007-2].

Roberts, R. D., Beh, H. C., and Stankov, L. (1988). Hick’s law, competing-task performance, and intelligence. Intelligence, 12, 111–30 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(88)90011-6].

Roberts, R. D., Markham, P. M., Matthews, G., and Zeidner, M. (2005). Assessing intelligence: Past, present, and future. In Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence (eds O. Wilhelm and R. W. Engle). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Rode, J. C., Mooney, C. H., Arthaud-Day, M. L., et  al. (2008). An examination of the structural, discriminant, nomological, and incremental predictive validity of the MSCEIT© V 2. 0. Intelligence, 36, 350–66.

Rodgers, J. L. and Rowe, D. C. (1985). Does contiguity breed similarity? A within-family analysis of non-shared sources of IQ differences between siblings. Developmental Psychology, 21, 743–6 [DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.21.5.743].

Rodgers, J. L., Cleveland, H. H., van den Oord, E., and Rowe, D. C. (2000). Resolving the debate over birth order, family size, and intelligence. American Psychologist, 55, 599–612 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.6.599] [PubMed: 10892201].

Rodríguez, C. A., Torres, A. A., Mackintosh, N. J., and Chamizo, V. D. (2010). Sex differences in the strategies used by rats to solve a navigation task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 36, 395–401 [DOI: 10.1037/a0017297] [PubMed: 20658870].

Roe, A. (1953). A psychological study of eminent psychologists and anthropologists, and a comparison with biological and physical scientists. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 67, Whole No. 353.

Roid, G. H. (2003). Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale (5th edn). Itasca, IL: Riverside.

Romine, C. B. and Reynolds, C. R. (2005). A model of the development of frontal lobe functioning: Findings from a meta-analysis. Applied Neuropsychology, 12, 190–201 [DOI: 10.1207/s15324826an1204_2] [PubMed: 16422660].

Roof, R. L. and Havens, M. D. (1992). Testosterone improves maze performance and induces development of a male hippocampus in females. Brain Research, 572, 310–3 [DOI: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90491-Q] [PubMed: 1611529].

Rose, S., Kamin, L. J., and Lewontin, R. C. (1984). Not in our genes. London: Penguin Books.

Rosenthal, R. (1994). Interpersonal expectancy effects: A 30-year perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 176–9 [DOI: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770698].

Rosenthal, R. and Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., Di Matteo, M. R., Rogers, P. L., and Archer, D. (1979). Sensitivity to nonverbal communication: The PONS Test. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Roth, E. (1964). Die Geschwindigheit der Verarbeitung von Information und ihr Zusammenhang mit Intelligenz. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 11, 616–22.

Royer, J. M. and Garofoli, L. M. (2005). Cognitive contributions to sex differences in math performance. In Gender differences in mathematics: An integrative psychological approach (eds A. M. Gallagher and J. C. Kaufman). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ruff, C. C., Knauff, M., Fangmeier, T., and Spreer, J. (2003). Reasoning and working memory: Common and distinct neuronal processes. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1241–53 [DOI: 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00016-2] [PubMed: 12753963].

Rush, D., Stein, Z., and Susser, M. (1980). Diet in pregnancy: A randomized controlled trial of nutritional supplements. New York: Liss.

Rushton, J. P. (1992). Cranial capacity related to sex, rank and race in a stratified random sample of 6,325 US military personnel. Intelligence, 16, 401–13 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(92)90017-L].

Rushton, J. P. (1995). Race, evolution and behavior: A life history perspective. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishing.

Rushton, J. P. (1999). Secular gains in IQ not related to the g factor and inbreeding depression-— unlike black-white differences: A reply to Flynn. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 381–9.

Rushton, J. P. and Ankney, C. D. (1996). Brain size and cognitive ability: correlations with age, sex, social class and race. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3, 21–36 [DOI: 10.3758/BF03210739].

Rushton, J. P. and Jensen, A. R. (2005). Thirty years of research on race differences in cognitive ability. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 11, 235–94 [DOI: 10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235].

Russell, J. (1996). Agency: Its role in mental development. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Ruthsatz, J., Detterman, D., Griscom, W. S., and Cirullo, B. A. (2008). Becoming an expert in the musical domain: It takes more than just practice. Intelligence, 36, 330–8 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2007.08.003].

Rutter, M. (2000). Discussion. In The nature of intelligence. (Novartis Foundation Symposium 233, 217);, (eds G. R. Bock, J. A. Goode, and K. Webb). Chichester: Wiley.

Rutter, M., Tizard, J., and Whitmore, K. (1970). Education, health and behaviour. London: Longman.

Back to top

Saccuzzo, D. P., Larson, G. E., and Rimland, B. (1986). Visual, auditory and reaction time approaches to the measurement of speed of information processing and individual differences in intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 659–67 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(86)90035-8].

Sackett, P. R., Borneman, M. J., and Connelly, B. S. (2008). High stakes testing in higher education and employment: Appraising the evidence for validity and fairness. American Psychologist, 63, 215–27 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.63.4.215] [PubMed: 18473607].

Sackett, P. R., Hardison, C. M., and Cullen, M. J. (2004). On interpreting stereotype threat as accounting for African-American white differences on cognitive tests. American Psychologist, 59, 7–13 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.7] [PubMed: 14736315].

Salgado, J. F., Anderson, N., Moscoso, S., et  al. (2003). A meta-analytic study of general mental ability validity for different occupations in the European community. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 1068–81 [DOI: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.6.1068] [PubMed: 14640817].

Salthouse, T. A. (1992). Mechanisms of age-cognition relations in adulthood. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–28 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.403] [PubMed: 8759042].

Salthouse, T. A., Atkinson, T. M., and Berish, D. E. (2003). Executive functioning as a potential mediator of age-related cognitive decline in normal adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 566–94 [DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.4.566].

Samelson, F. (1975). On the science and politics of IQ. Social Research, 42, 467–88.

Samelson, F. (1979). Putting psychology on the map: Ideology and intelligence testing. In Psychology in social context (ed. A. Buss). New York: Wiley.

Sandstrom, N. J., Kaufman, J., and Huettel, S. A. (1998). Males and females use different distal cues in a virtual environment navigation task. Cognitive Brain Research, 6, 351–60 [DOI: 10.1016/S0926-6410(98)00002-0] [PubMed: 9593991].

Sarason, S. B., and Doris, J. (1979). Educational handicap, public policy, and social history. New York: Free Press.

Saucier, G. (1992). Openness versus intellect: Much ado about nothing? European Journal of Personality, 6, 381–6 [DOI: 10.1002/per.2410060506].

Saucier, D. M., Green, S. M., Leason, J., et  al. (2002). Are sex differences in navigation caused by sexually dimorphic strategies or by differences in the ability to use the strategies? Behavioral Neuroscience, 116, 403–10 [DOI: 10.1037/0735-7044.116.3.403] [PubMed: 12049321].

Scarr, S. (1997). Behavior—genetic and socialization theories of intelligence: Truce and reconciliation. In Intelligence: Heredity and environment (eds R. J. Sternberg and E. Grigorenko). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Scarr, S. and McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype → environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424–35 [DOI: 10.2307/1129703] [PubMed: 6683622].

Scarr, S. and Weinberg, R. A. (1976). IQ test performance of black children adopted by white families. American Psychologist, 31, 726–39 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.31.10.726].

Scarr, S., and Weinberg, R. A. (1978). The influence of ‘family background’ on intellectual attainment. American Sociological Review, 43, 674–92 [DOI: 10.2307/2094543].

Scarr, S. and Weinberg, R. A. (1983). The Minnesota adoption studies: Genetic differences and malleability. Child Development, 54, 260–7 [DOI: 10.2307/1129689] [PubMed: 6872626].

Scarr, S., Pakstis, A. J., Katz, S. H., and Barker, W. B. (1977). Absence of a relationship between degree of white ancestry and intellectual skills within a black population. Human Genetics, 39, 69–86 [DOI: 10.1007/BF00273154] [PubMed: 562835].

Schaie, K. W. (2005). Developmental influences on adult intelligence: The Seattle longitudinal study. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Schama, S. (1989). Citizens. New York: Knopf.

Schmidt, F. L. and Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection methods in personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 262–74 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.262].

Schneider, R. J., Ackerman, P. L., and Kanfer, R. (1996). To ‘act wisely in human relations’: Exploring the dimensions of social competence. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 469–81 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(96)00084-0].

Schneider, W., Gruber, H., Gold, A., and Opwis, K. (1993). Chess expertise and memory for chess positions in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 328–49 [DOI: 10.1006/jecp.1993.1038] [PubMed: 8301242].

Schoenemann, P. T., Budinger, T. F., Sarich, V. M., and Wang, W. S. Y. (2000). Brain size does not predict general cognitive ability within families. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 4932–7 [DOI: 10.1073/pnas.97.9.4932].

Schoenthaler, S. J., Amos, S. P., Eysenck, H. J., Peritz, E., and Yudkin, J. (1991). Controlled trial of vitamin-mineral supplementation: Effects on intelligence and performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 351–62 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(91)90287-L].

Schull, W. J. and Neel, J. V. (1965). The effects of inbreeding on Japanese children. New York: Harper and Row.

Schulte, M. J., Ree, M. J., and Carretta, T. R. (2004). Emotional intelligence: Not much more than g and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1059–68 [DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2003.11.014].

Schulze, R. (2005). Modeling structures of intelligence. In Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence (eds O. Wilhelm and R. W. Engle). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., et  al. (1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 167–77 [DOI: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00001-4].

Schwartzman, A. E., Gold, D., Andres, D., Arbuckle, T. Y., and Chaikelson, J. (1987). Stability of intelligence: A 40-year follow-up. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, 244–56 [DOI: 10.1037/h0084155] [PubMed: 3502899].

Schweinhart, L. J. and Weikart, D. P. (1980). Young children grow up: The effects of the Perry Preschool Program on youths through age 15. Monographs of the High/Scope Educational Research Foundation No.  7.

Scottish Council for Research in Education (1949). The trend of Scottish intelligence. London: London University Press.

Scribner, S. (1986). Thinking in action: Some characteristics of practical thought. In Practical intelligence: Nature and origins of competence in the everyday world (eds R. J. Sternebrg and R. K. Wagner). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Seifer, R. (2001). Socioeconomic status, multiple risks, and development of intelligence. In Environmental effects on cognitive abilities (eds R. J. Sternberg and E. L. Grigorenko). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Serpell, R. (2000). Intelligence and culture. In Handbook of intelligence (ed. R. J. Sternberg). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Shah, P. and Miyake, A. (1996). The separability of working memory resources for spatial thinking and language processing: An individual differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 4–27 [DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.125.1.4].

Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Shallice, T. (2002). Fractionation of the supervisory system. In Principles of frontal lobe function (eds D. T. Stuss and R. T. Knight). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Shallice, T. and Burgess, P. (1998). The domain of supervisory processes and the temporal organization of behaviour. In The prefrontal cortex: Executive and cognitive functions (eds A. C. Roberts, T. W. Robbins, and L. Weiskrantz). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Shanks, D. R. (2005). Implicit learning. In Handbook of cognition (eds K. Lamberts and R. Goldstone). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Shanks, D. R., Green, R. E. A., and Kolodny, J. A. (1994). A critical examination of the evidence for unconscious (implicit) learning. In Attention and performance, XV (eds C. Umilta and M. Moscovitch). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Sharma, R. (1971). Unpublished Ph.D. Thesis. University of London.

Shaw, P., Greenstein, D., Lerch, J., et  al. (2006). Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature, 440, 676–9 [DOI: 10.1038/nature04513] [PubMed: 16572172].

Shayer, M. and Ginsburg, D. (2009). Thirty years on—a large anti-Flynn effect? (II): 13 and 14-year-olds. Piagetian tests of formal operations norms 1976–2006/7. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 409–18.

Shayer, M., Ginsburg, D., and Coe, R. (2007). Thirty years on—a large anti-Flynn effect? The Piagetian test Volume & Heaviness norms 1975–2003. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 25–41 [DOI: 10.1348/000709906X96987] [PubMed: 17411486].

Shepard, R. N. (1978). The mental image. American Psychologist, 33, 125–7 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.33.2.125].

Shepard, R. N. and Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701–3 [DOI: 10.1126/science.171.3972.701] [PubMed: 5540314].

Shettleworth, S. J. (2010). Cognition, evolution, and behavior (2nd edn). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Shields, J. (1962). Monozygotic twins brought up apart and brought up together. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Shuey, A. M. (1966). The testing of negro intelligence (2nd edn). New York: Social Science Press.

Siegler, R. S. and Alibali, M. W. (2005). Children’s thinking (4th edn). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Sigman, M. and Whaley, S. E. (1998). The role of nutrition in the development of intelligence. In The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures (ed. U. Neisser). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Silverman, I. and Eals, M. (1992). Sex differences in spatial abilities: Evolutionary theory and data. In The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (eds J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Silverman, I., Choi, J., and Peters, M. (2007). The hunter-gatherer theory of sex differences in spatial abilities: Data from 40 countries. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 261–8 [DOI: 10.1007/s10508-006-9168-6] [PubMed: 17351740].

Silverstein, A. B. (1982). Factor structure of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 661–4 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-006X.50.5.661].

Simonton, D. K. (1991). Emergence and realization of genius: The lives and works of 120 classical composers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 829–40 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.5.829].

Simonton, D. K. (2003). Genius and g: Intelligence and exceptional achievement. In The scientific study of general intelligence: Tribute to Arthur R. Jensen (ed. H. Nyborg). Oxford: Pergamon.

Skuse, D. (1984). Extreme deprivation in early childhood—II. Theoretical issues and a comparative review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 893–901.

Slabbekoorn, D., van Goozen, S. H. M., Megens, J., Gooren, L. J. G., and Cohen-Kettenis, P. T. (1999). Activating effects of cross-sex hormones on cognitive functioning: A study of short-term and long-term hormone effects in transsexuals. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 24, 423–47 [DOI: 10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00091-2] [PubMed: 10341369].

Smith, E. E., Jonides, J., and Koeppe, R. A. (1996). Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 11–20 [DOI: 10.1093/cercor/6.1.11] [PubMed: 8670634].

Smith, E. E., Jonides, J., Koeppe, R. A., Awh, E., Schumacher, E. H., and Minoshima, S. (1995). Spatial versus object working-memory—PET investigations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 337–56 [DOI: 10.1162/jocn.1995.7.3.337].

Smyth, M. M. and Scholey, K. A. (1992). Determining spatial span: The role of movement time and articulation rate. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 479–501.

Smyth, M. M. and Scholey, K. A. (1994). Interference in immediate spatial memory. Memory and Cognition, 22, 1–13 [DOI: 10.3758/BF03202756].

Snow, R. E. (1995). Pygmalion and intelligence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 169–71 [DOI: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772605].

Snow, R. E., Kyllonen, P. C., and Marshalek, B. . 1984)(1984). The topography of ability and learning correlations. In Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (vol. 2), (ed. R. J. Sternberg). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Snyderman, M. and Herrnstein, R. J. (1983). Intelligence tests and the Immigration Act of 1924. American Psychologist, 38, 986–95 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.38.9.986].

Snyderman, M. and Rothman, S. (1988). The IQ controversy: The media and public policy. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.

Sokol, D. K., Moore, C. A., Rose, R. J., and Williams, C. J. (1995). Intrapair differences in personality and cognitive ability among young monozygotic twins distinguished by chorion type. Behavior Genetics, 25, 457–66 [DOI: 10.1007/BF02253374] [PubMed: 7487842].

Solomon, M. (1995). Mozart: A.  life. London: Hutchinson.

Sommer, R. and Sommer, B. A. (1983). Mystery in Milwaukee: Early intervention, IQ, and psychology textbooks. American Psychologist, 38, 982–5 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.38.9.982] [PubMed: 6638665].

Song, L. J., Huang, G.-H., Peng, K. Z., et  al. (2010). The differential effects of general mental ability and emotional intelligence on academic performance and social interactions. Intelligence, 38, 137–43 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2009.09.003].

Sowell, T. (1978). Essays and data on American ethnic groups. Washington, DC: The Urban Institute.

Sowell, T. (1981). Ethnic America: A history. New York: Basic Books.

Spearman, C. (1904). General intelligence, objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15, 201–93 [DOI: 10.2307/1412107].

Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man. London: Macmillan.

Spearman, C. and Jones, L. L. (1950). Human ability. London: Macmillan.

Spelke, E. S. (2005). Sex differences in intrinsic aptitude for mathematics and science? A critical review. American Psychologist, 60, 950–8 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.9.950] [PubMed: 16366817].

Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., and Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women’s math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4–28 [DOI: 10.1006/jesp.1998.1373].

Spitz, H. H. (1986). The raising of intelligence: A selected history of attempts to raise retarded intelligence. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Stafford, R. E. (1961). Sex differences in spatial visualization as evidence of sex-linked inheritance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 13, 428 [DOI: 10.2466/PMS.13.7.428-428].

Stanford, W. B. and Luce, J. V. (1974). The quest for Ulysses. London: Phaidon Press.

Stankov, L. and Crawford, J. D. (1993). Ingredients of complexity in fluid intelligence. Learning and Individual Differences, 5, 73–111 [DOI: 10.1016/1041-6080(93)90007-F].

Stanovich, K. E. and West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645–65 [DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X00003435] [PubMed: 11301544].

Staszewski, J. J. (1990). Exceptional memory: The influence of practice and knowledge on the development of elaborative encoding strategies. In Interactions among aptitudes, strategies, and knowledge in cognitive performance (eds W. Schneider and F. E. Wenert). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Steele, C. M. and Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and  the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797–811 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797] [PubMed: 7473032].

Stein, Z., Susser, M., Saenger, G., and Marolla, F. (1972). Nutrition and mental performance. Science, 178, 708–13 [DOI: 10.1126/science.178.4062.708] [PubMed: 5082838].

Stelzl, I., Merz, F., Ehlers, T., and Remer, H. (1995). The effect of schooling on the development of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A quasi-experimental study. Intelligence, 21, 279–96 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(95)90018-7].

Stern, W. (1912). Die Psychologische Methoden der Intelligenzprüfung. Leipzig: Barth.

Sternberg, R. J. (1977). Intelligence, information processing and analogical reasoning: The componential analysis of human abilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Sternberg, R. J. (1986). Toward a unified theory of human reasoning. Intelligence, 10, 281–314 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(86)90001-2].

Sternberg, R. J. (1990). Metaphors of mind: Conceptions of the nature of intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Successful intelligence. New York: Plume.

Sternberg, R. J. and Grigorenko, E. L. (eds) (2001). Environmental effects on cognitive abilities, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Sternberg, R. J. and Grigorenko, E. L. (eds) (2002). The general factor of intelligence: How general is it? Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Sternberg, R. J. and Powell, J. S. (1983). Comprehending verbal comprehension. American Psychologist, 38, 878–93 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.38.8.878].

Sternberg, R. J. and Rifkin, B. (1979). The development of analogical reasoning processes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 195–232 [DOI: 10.1016/0022-0965(79)90044-4] [PubMed: 438723].

Sternberg, R. J. and Smith, C. (1985). Social intelligence and decoding skills in nonverbal communication. Social Cognition, 3, 168–92 [DOI: 10.1521/soco.1985.3.2.168].

Sternberg, R. J. and Wagner, R. K. (eds) (1986). Practical intelligence: Nature and origins of competence in the everyday world. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Sternberg, R. J. and Weil, E. M. (1980). An aptitude-strategy interaction in linear syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 226–34 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.72.2.226].

Sternberg, R. J., Conway, B. E., Ketron, J. L., and Bernstein, M. (1981). People’s conceptions of intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 37–55 [DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.41.1.37].

Sternberg, R. J., Forsythe, G. B., Hedlund, J., et  al. (2000). Practical intelligence in everyday life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Sternberg, R. J., Wagner, R. K., Williams, W. M., and Horvath, J. A. (1995). Testing common sense. American Psychologist, 50, 912–26 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.50.11.912].

Sternberg, S. (1975). Memory scanning: new findings and current controversies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17, 1–32.

Stevenson, H. W., Lee, S., Chen, C., and Stigler, J. W. (1990). Contexts of achievement: A study of American, Chinese, and Japanese children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 55.

Stevenson, H. W., Stigler, J. W., Lee, S., Lucker, G. W., Kitanawa, S., and Hsu, C. (1985). Cognitive performance and academic achievement of Japanese, Chinese and American children. Child Development, 56, 718–34 [DOI: 10.2307/1129761].

Stoolmiller, M. (1999). Implications of the restricted range of family environments for estimates of heritability and nonshared environment in behavior—genetic adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 392–409 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.4.392] [PubMed: 10414224].

Storfer, M. D. (1990). Intelligence and giftedness: The contributions of heredity and early environment. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Stough, C. K. K., Nettelbeck, T., and Cooper, C. J. (1990). Evoked brain potentials, string length and intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 401–6 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(90)90223-E].

Strand, S., Deary, I. J., and Smith, P. (2006). Sex differences in Cognitive Abilities Test scores: A UK national picture. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 463–80 [DOI: 10.1348/000709905X50906] [PubMed: 16953957].

Streissguth, A. P., Barr, H. M., Sampson, P. D., Darby, B. L., and Martin, O. C. (1990). IQ at age 4 in relation to maternal alcohol use and smoking during pregnancy. Developmental Psychology, 25, 3–11.

Strenze, T. (2007). Intelligence and socioeconomic success: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal research. Intelligence, 35, 401–26 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.09.004].

Stumpf, H., and Eliot, J. (1995). Gender related differences in spatial ability and the K factor of general spatial ability in a population of academically talented students. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 33–45 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(95)00029-6].

Sundet, J. M., Barlaug, D. G., and Torjussen, T. M. (2004). The end of the Flynn effect? A study of secular trends in mean intelligence test scores of Norwegian conscripts during half a century. Intelligence, 32, 349–62 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(04)00052-2].

Sundet, J. M., Borren, I., and Tambs, K. (2008). The Flynn effect is partly caused by changing fertility patterns. Intelligence, 36, 183–91 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2007.04.002].

Suss, H.-M. and Beauducel, A. (2005). Faceted models of intelligence. In Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence (eds O. Wilhelm and R. W. Engle). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Suss, H.-M., Oberauer, K., Wittmann, W. W., Wilhelm, O., and Schulze, R. (2002). Working-memory capacity explains reasoning ability—and a little bit more. Intelligence, 30, 261–88 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00100-3].

Back to top

Tambs, K., Sundet, J. M., and Magnus, P. (1984). Heritability analysis of the WAIS subtests: A study of twins. Intelligence, 8, 283–93 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(84)90012-6].

Tamez, E., Myerson, J., and Hale, S. (2008). Learning, working memory, and intelligence revisited. Behavioural Processes, 78, 240–5 [DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.01.008] [PubMed: 18308483].

Tang, C. Y., Eaves, E. L., Ng, J. C., et  al. (2010). Brain networks for working memory and factors of intelligence assessed in males and females with fMRI and DTI. Intelligence, 38, 293–303 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2010.03.003].

Taylor, H. F. (1980). The IQ game: A methodological inquiry into the heredity-environment controversy. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

Teasdale, T. W. and Owen, D. R. (2000). Forty-year secular trends in cognitive abilitiesabilties. Intelligence, 28, 115–20 [DOI: 10.1016/S0160-2896(99)00034-3].

Teasdale, T. W. and Owen, D. R. (2008). Secular declines in cognitive test scores: A reversal of the Flynn Effect. Intelligence, 36, 121–6 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2007.01.007].

Teichner, W. H. and Krebs, M. J. (1974). Laws of visual choice reaction time. Psychological Review, 81, 75–98 [DOI: 10.1037/h0035867] [PubMed: 4812881].

Terman, L. M. (1916). The measurement of intelligence. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Terman, L. M. (1932). An autobiography. In A history of  psychology in autobiography (vol. 2.), (ed. C. Murchison). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.

Terman, L. M. and Merrill, M. A. (1937). Measuring intelligence. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Terman, L. M., and Oden, M. H. (1947). The gifted child grows up. Vol 4. Genetic studies of genius. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Terman, L. M., and Oden, M. H. (1959). The gifted group at mid-life. Vol 5. Genetic studies of genius. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Teuber, H. L. (1964). The riddle of frontal lobe function in man. In The frontal granular cortex and behaviour (eds J. M. Warren and K. Albert). New York: McGraw-Hill.

Thompson, P. M., Cannon, T. D., Narr, K. L., et  al. (2001). Genetic influences on brain structure. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 1253–8 [DOI: 10.1038/nn758] [PubMed: 11694885].

Thomson, G. H. (1916). A hierarchy without a general factor. British Journal of Psychology, 8, 271–81.

Thomson, G. H. (1921). The Northumberland Mental Tests. British Journal of Psychology, 12, 201–22.

Thomson, G. H. (1939). The factorial analysis of human ability. London: University of London Press.

Thorndike, E. L. (1920). Intelligence and its use. Harper’s Magazine, 140, 227–35.

Thorndike, E. L. (1925). The measurement of intelligence. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.

Thorndike, R. L. (1987). Stability of factor loadings. Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 585–6 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(87)90224-8].

Thurstone, L. L. (1931). Multiple factor analysis. Psychological Review, 38, 406–27 [DOI: 10.1037/h0069792].

Thurstone, L. L. (1938). Primary mental abilities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Thurstone, L. L. (1940). The vectors of the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Thurstone, L. L. (1947). Multiple factor analysis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Thurstone, L. L. and Thurstone, T. G. (1941). Factorial studies of intelligence. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Tideman, E. and Gustafsson, J.-E. (2004) Age-related differentiation of cognitive abilities in ages 3–7. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1965–74.

Till, N. (1992). Mozart and the enlightenment. London: Faber and Faber.

Tishkoff, S. A., Ree, F. A., Friedlaender, F. R. et  al. (2009). The genetic structure and history of Africans and African Americans. Science, 324, 1035–44 [DOI: 10.1126/science.1172257].

Tizard, B. (1974). IQ and race. Nature, 247, 316 [DOI: 10.1038/247316a0] [PubMed: 4818369].

Towse, J. N. and Hitch, G. J. (2008). Variation in working memory due to normal development. In Variation in working memory (eds A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. Kane, A. Miyake, and J. Towse). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Towse, J. N., Hitch, G. J., and Hutton, U. (1998). A re-evaluation of working memory capacity in children. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 195–217 [DOI: 10.1006/jmla.1998.2574].

Tresch, M. C., Sinnamon, H. M., and Seamon, J. C. (1993). Double dissociation of spatial and object visual memory: Evidence from selective interference in intact human subjects. Neuropsychologia, 31, 211–9 [DOI: 10.1016/0028-3932(93)90085-E] [PubMed: 8492874].

Tuddenham, R. D. (1948). Soldier intelligence in World Wars I and II. American Psychologist, 3, 54–6 [DOI: 10.1037/h0054962] [PubMed: 18911933].

Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D’Onofrio, B., and Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science, 14, 623–8 [DOI: 10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x] [PubMed: 14629696].

Back to top

Underwood, B. J., Boruch, R. F., and Malmi, R. A. (1978). Composition of episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 107, 393–419 [DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.107.4.393].

Unsworth, N. and Engle, R. W. (2006). Simple and complex memory spans and their relation to fluid abilities: Evidence from list-length effects. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 68–80 [DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.06.003].

Unsworth, N. and Engle, R. W. (2007). On the division of short-term and working memory: An examination of simple and complex span and their relation to higher order abilities. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1038–66 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.6.1038] [PubMed: 17967093].

Unsworth, N., Redick, T. S., Lakey, C. E., and Young, D. L. (2010) Lapses in sustained attention and their relation to executive control and fluid abilities: An individual differences investigation. Intelligence, 38, 111–22 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2009.08.002].

Unterrainer, J. M., Kaller, C. P., Halsband, U., and Rahm, B. (2006). Planning abilities and chess: A comparison of chess and non-chess players on the Tower of London task. British Journal of Psychology, 97, 299–311 [DOI: 10.1348/000712605X71407] [PubMed: 16848944].

Back to top

VanCourt, M. and Bean, F. D. (1985). Intelligence and fertility in the United States: 1912–1982. Intelligence, 9, 23–32 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(85)90004-2].

Van Daalen-Kapteijns, M. M., and Elshout-Mohr, M. (1981). The acquisition of word meanings as a cognitive learning process. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 20, 386–99 [DOI: 10.1016/S0022-5371(81)90515-6].

van der Maas, H. L. J., Dolan, C. V., Grasman, R. P. P. P., et  al. (2006). A dynamical model of general intelligence: The positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychological Review, 113, 842–61 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.4.842] [PubMed: 17014305].

van IJzendoorn, M. H., Juffer, F., and Poelhuis, C. W. K. (2005). Adoption and cognitive development: A meta-analytic comparison of adopted and nonadopted children’s IQ and school performance. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 301–16 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.301] [PubMed: 15740423].

van Leeuwen, M., Peper, J. S., van den Berg, S. M., et  al. (2009). A genetic analysis of brain volumes and IQ in children. Intelligence, 37, 181–91 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.10.005].

van Praag, H., Kempermann, G., and Gage, F. H. (2000). Neural consequences of environmental enrichment. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1, 191–8 [DOI: 10.1038/35042057] [PubMed: 11257907].

Van Valen, L. (1974). Brain size and intelligence in man. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 40, 417–24 [DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.1330400314] [PubMed: 4596955].

Van Wieringen, J. C. (1978). Secular growth changes. In Human growth (vol. 2), (eds F. Falkner and J. M. Tanner). New York: Plenum Press.

Vandierendonck, A. and De Vooght, G. (1997). Working memory constraints on linear reasoning with spatial and temporal contents. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 50A, 803–20.

Vecchi, T. (1998). Visuo-spatial imagery in congenitally totally blind people. Memory, 6, 91–102 [DOI: 10.1080/741941601] [PubMed: 9640434].

Vendrell, P., Junque, C., Pujol, J., and Jurado, M. A. (1995). The role of prefrontal regions in the Stroop task. Neuropsychologia, 33, 341–52 [DOI: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)00116-7] [PubMed: 7792000].

Verhaeghen, P. (2003). Aging and vocabulary score: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 18, 332–9 [DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.18.2.332] [PubMed: 12825780].

Vernon, P. A. (1993). Der Zahlen-Verbindungs-Test and other trail-making correlates of general intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 35–40 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(93)90172-Y].

Vernon, P. A. and Jensen, A. R. (1984). Individual and group differences in intelligence and speed of information processing. Personality and Individual Differences, 5, 911–23.

Vernon, P. A. and Weese, S. E. (1993). Predicting intelligence with multiple speed of information-processing tests. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 413–19 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(93)90310-Y].

Vernon, P. A., Nador, S., and Cantor, L. (1985). Group differences in intelligence and speed of information-processing. Intelligence, 9, 137–48 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(85)90013-3].

Vernon, P. E. (1947). Research on personnel selection in the Royal Navy and British Army. American Psychologist, 2, 35–51 [DOI: 10.1037/h0056920] [PubMed: 20288871].

Vernon, P. E. (1950). The structure of human abilities. London: Methuen.

Vernon, P. E. (1957). Secondary school selection. A British psychological inquiry. London: Methuen.

Vernon, P. E. (1969). Intelligence and cultural environment. London: Methuen.

Vernon, P. E. (1982). The abilities and achievements of Orientals in North America. New York: Academic Press.

Vickers, D. (1979). Decision processes in visual perception. London: Academic Press.

Vigneau, F. and Bors, D. A. (2008). The quest for item types based on information processing: An analysis of Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices, with a consideration of gender differences. Intelligence, 36, 702–10 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.04.004].

Villardita, C. (1985). Raven’s Colored Progressive Matrices and intellectual impairment in patients with focal brain damage. Cortex, 21, 627–35 [PubMed: 2419033].

Vining, D. R. (1995). On the possibility of the re-emergence of a dysgenic trend with respect to intelligence in American fertility differentials: An update. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 259–63 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(95)00038-8].

Visser, B. A., Ashton, M. C., and Vernon, P. A. (2006). Beyond g: Putting multiple intelligences theory to the test. Intelligence, 34, 487–502 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.02.004].

Vogel, F., Kruger, J., Schalt, E., Schnobel, R., and Hassling. L. (1987). No consistent relationships between oscillations and latencies of visual evoked EEG potentials and measures of mental performance. Human Neurobiology, 6, 173–82 [PubMed: 3449486].

Voyer, D., Voyer, S., and Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–70 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.250] [PubMed: 7724690].

Back to top

Wager, T. D. and Smith, E. E. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of working memory: A meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 255–74 [DOI: 10.3758/CABN.3.4.255].

Wagner, G., Koch, K., Reichenbach, J. R., Sauer, H., and Schlösser, R. G. M. (2006). The special involvement of the rostrolateral prefrontal cortex in planning abilities: An event-related fMRI study with the Tower of London paradigm. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2337–47 [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.014] [PubMed: 16806311].

Wagner, R. K., and Sternberg, R. J. (1986). Tacit knowledge and intelligence in the everyday world. In Practical intelligence: Nature and origins of competence in the everyday world (eds R. J. Sternberg and R. K. Wagner). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Waiter, G. D., Deary, I. J., Staff, R. T., et  al. (2009). Exploring possible neural mechanisms of intelligence differences using processing speed and working memory tasks: An fMRI study. Intelligence, 37, 199–206 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.09.008].

Walhovd, K. B., Fjell, A. M., Reinvang, I., et  al. (2005). Cortical volume and speed-of-processing are complementary in prediction of performance intelligence. Neuropsychologia, 43, 704–13 [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.08.006] [PubMed: 15721183].

Waller, J. H. (1971). Achievement and social mobility: Relationships among IQ score, education and occupation in two generations. Social Biology, 18, 252–9 [PubMed: 5120877].

Waltz, J. A., Knowlton, B. J., Holyoak, K. J., et  al. (1999). A system for relational reasoning in human prefrontal cortex. Psychological Science, 10, 119–25 [DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.00118].

Wartenburger, I., Heekeren, H. R., Preusse, F., Kramer, J., and van der Meer, E. (2009). Cerebral correlates of analogical processing and their modulation by training. NeuroImage, 48, 291–302 [DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.025] [PubMed: 19539767].

Washburn, D. A. and Rumbaugh, D. M. (1997). Faster is smarter, so why are we slower? A comparative perspective on intelligence and processing speed. American Psychologist, 52, 1147–8 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.10.1147].

Wason, P. (1968). Reasoning about a rule. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 273–81 [DOI: 10.1080/14640746808400161] [PubMed: 5683766].

Wasserman, G. A. and Factor-Litvak, P. (2001). Methodology, inference and causation: Environmental lead exposure and childhood intelligence. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, 343–52 [DOI: 10.1016/S0887-6177(00)00085-8] [PubMed: 14590166].

Waters, A. J., Gobet, F., and Leyden, G. (2002). Visuospatial abilities of chess players. British Journal of Psychology, 93, 557–65 [DOI: 10.1348/000712602761381402] [PubMed: 12519534].

Watkins, M. W., Lei, P.-W., and Canivez, G. L. (2007). Psychometric intelligence and achievement: A cross-lagged panel analysis. Intelligence, 35, 59–68 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.04.005].

Wechsler, D. (1944). Measurement of adult intelligence (3rd edn). Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.

Wechsler, D. (1958). The measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence (4th edn). Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.

Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—3rd edition (WAIS-III). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—4th edition (WISC-IV). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—4th edition (WAIS-IV). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Weinberg, R. A., Scarr, S., and Waldman, I. D. (1992). The Minnesota transracial adoption study: A follow-up of IQ test performance at adolescence. Intelligence, 16, 117–35 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(92)90028-P].

Weinstein, R. S., Gregory, A., and Strambler, M. J. (2004). Intractable self-fulfilling prophecies fifty years after Brown vs. Board of Education. American Psychologist, 59, 511–20 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.6.511] [PubMed: 15367086].

Weisberg, R. W. (2006). Modes of expertise in creative thinking: Evidence from case studies. In Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (eds K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. Feltovich, and R. R. Hoffman). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Weiss, E. M., Ragland, J. D., Brensinger, C. M., et  al. (2006). Sex difference in clustering and switching verbal fluency tasks. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 502–9 [PubMed: 16981602].

West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 272–92 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.2.272] [PubMed: 8831298].

Weyl, N. (1969). Some comparative performance indexes of American ethnic minorities. Mankind Quarterly, 9, 106–19.

Whalley, L. J. and Deary, I. J. (2001). Longitudinal cohort study of childhood IQ and survival up to age 76. British Medical Journal, 322, 819–22.

White, K. R. (1982). The relation between socioeconomic status and academic achievement. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 461–81 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.91.3.461].

Whiten, A. and Byrne, R. W. (1988). Tactical deception in primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 233–73 [DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X00049682].

Wicherts, J. M., Dolan, C. V., and van der Maas, H. L. J. (2010). A systematic literature review of the average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans. Intelligence, 38, 1–20 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2009.05.002].

Wicherts, J. M., Dolan, C. V., Hessen, D. J., et  al. (2004). Are intelligence tests measurement invariant over time? Investigating the nature of the Flynn effect. Intelligence, 32, 509–37 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.002].

Wickett, J. C., Vernon, P. A., and Lee, D. H. (2000). Relationships between factors of intelligence and brain volume. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 1095–122 [DOI: 10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00258-5].

Widaman, K. F. and Carlson, J. S. (1989). Procedural effects on performance in the Hick paradigm: Bias in reaction time and movement time parameters. Intelligence, 13, 63–85 [DOI: 10.1016/0160-2896(89)90007-X].

Wilhelm, O. (2005). Measuring reasoning ability. In Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence (eds O. Wilhelm and R. W. Engle). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Wilhelm, O. and Schulze, R. (2002). The relation of speeded and unspeeded reasoning with mental speed. Intelligence, 30, 537–54.

Willerman, L., Naylor, A. F., and Myrianthopoulos, N. C. (1974). Intellectual development of children from interracial matings: Performance in infancy and at 4 years. Behavior Genetics, 4, 83–90 [DOI: 10.1007/BF01066706] [PubMed: 4816569].

Williams, B. A. and Pearlberg, S. L. (2006). Learning of three-term contingencies correlates with Raven scores, but not with measures of cognitive processing. Intelligence, 34, 177–91 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2005.03.007].

Williams, C. L. and Meck, W. H. (1991). The organizational effects of gonadal steroids on sexually dimorphic spatial ability. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 16, 155–76 [DOI: 10.1016/0306-4530(91)90076-6] [PubMed: 1961837].

Williams, W. M. (1998). Are we raising smarter children today? School and home-related influences on IQ. In The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures (ed. U. Neisser). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Willingham, D. B., Greeley, T., and Bardone, A. M. (1993). Dissociation in a serial response time task using a recognition measure: Comment on Perruchet and Amorin 1992 (1992). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 19, 1424–30.

Wilson, R. S. (1983). The Louisville twin study: Developmental synchronies in behaviour. Child Development, 54, 298–316 [DOI: 10.2307/1129693] [PubMed: 6683617].

Wilson, R. S. (1986). Continuity and change in cognitive ability profile. Behavior Genetics, 16, 45–60 [DOI: 10.1007/BF01065478] [PubMed: 3707487].

Wissler, C. (1901). The correlation of mental and physical tests. Psychological Review Monograph Supplement, 3, no. 6.

Woodrow, H. (1939). The common factors in fifty-two mental tests. Psychometrika, 4, 99–107 [DOI: 10.1007/BF02288489].

Woodrow, H. (1940). Interrelations of measures of learning. Journal of Psychology, 10, 49–73 [DOI: 10.1080/00223980.1940.9916984].

Wooldridge, A. (1994). Measuring the mind: Education and psychology in England, c. 1860–c. 1990. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Woolf, L. I., McBean, M. S., Woolf, F. M., and Cahalanf, S. F. (1975). Phenylketonuria as a balanced polymorphism: the nature of the heterozygote advantage. Annals of Human Genetics, 38, 461–9 [DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-1809.1975.tb00635.x].

Back to top

Yoakum, L. S. and Yerkes, R. M. (1920). Army mental tests. New York: Holt.

Back to top

Zacks, R. T., Hasher, L., and Li, K. Z. H. (2000). Human memory. In The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd edn), (eds F. I. M. Craik and T. A. Salthouse). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Zagorsky, J. L. (2007). Do you have to be smart to be rich? The impact of IQ on wealth, income and financial distress. Intelligence, 35, 489–501 [DOI: 10.1016/j.intell.2007.02.003].

Zajonc, R. B. (1983). Validating the confluence model. Psychological Bulletin, 93, 457–480 [DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.93.3.457].

Zajonc, R. B. and Mullally, P. R. (1997). Birth order: Reconciling conflicting effects. American Psychologist, 52, 685–99 [DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.7.685].

Zenderland, L. (1998). Measuring minds: Henry Herbert Goddard and the origins of American intelligence testing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Zhang, Y., Caryl, P. G., and Deary, I. J. (1989). Evoked potential correlates of inspection time. Personality and Individual Differences, 10, 379–84 [DOI: 10.1016/0191-8869(89)90002-0].

Zigler, E. and Muenchow, S. (1992). Head Start: The inside story of America’s most successful educational experiment. New York: Basic Books.

Zimmerman, W. S. (1954). Hypotheses concerning the nature of the spatial factors. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 14, 396–400 [DOI: 10.1177/001316445401400220].

Back to top